Encyclopedia, Difference between revisions of "B. F. Skinner" - New World

From New World Encyclopedia
Line 7: Line 7:
  
 
[[Image:Skinner.jpg|thumb|right|Burrhus Frederic Skinner]]
 
[[Image:Skinner.jpg|thumb|right|Burrhus Frederic Skinner]]
'''Burrhus Frederic Skinner''' (March 20, 1904 – August 18, 1990) was an [[United States|American]] [[psychologist]] and author. He conducted pioneering work on [[experimental psychology]] and advocated [[behaviorism]], which seeks to understand behavior as a function of environmental histories of reinforcement. He wrote a number of controversial works in which he proposed the widespread use of psychological behavior modification techniques, primarily using [[conditioning]], in order to improve society and increase human happiness, as a form of social engineering.
+
'''Burrhus Frederic Skinner''' (March 20, 1904 – August 18, 1990) was an [[United States|American]] [[psychologist]] and author. He conducted pioneering work in [[experimental psychology]] and advocated [[behaviorism]], which seeks to understand and control behavior as a function of environmental histories of reinforcement. Skinner alos wrote a number of controversial works in which he proposed the widespread use of psychological behavior modification techniques, primarily using [[conditioning]], in order to improve society and increase human happiness.
  
=Life=
+
==Life==
 
'''Burrhus Frederic Skinner''' was born in rural Susquehanna, Pennsylvania. He attended Hamilton College in New York, with the intention of becoming a writer and received a B.A. in English literature in 1926.  
 
'''Burrhus Frederic Skinner''' was born in rural Susquehanna, Pennsylvania. He attended Hamilton College in New York, with the intention of becoming a writer and received a B.A. in English literature in 1926.  
  
After graduation, he spent a year in Greenwich Village attempting to become a writer of [[fiction]], but he soon became disillusioned with his literary skills. He concluded that he had little world experience and no strong personal perspective from which to write. During this time, which Skinner later called "the dark year," he chanced upon a copy of [[Bertrand Russell]]'s recently published book ''An Outline of Philosophy'', in which Russell discussed the [[behaviorism|behaviorist]] philosophy of [[psychologist]] [[John B. Watson]]. At the time, Skinner had begun to take more interest in the actions and behaviors of those around him, and some of his short stories had taken a "psychological" slant. He decided to abandon literature and seek admission as a graduate student in psychology at [[Harvard University]] (which at the time was not regarded as a leading institution in the field).
+
After graduation, he spent a year in Greenwich Village attempting to become a writer of [[fiction]], but he soon became disillusioned with his literary skills. He concluded that he had little world experience and no strong personal perspective from which to write. During this time, which Skinner later called "the dark year," he chanced upon a copy of [[Bertrand Russell]]'s recently published book ''An Outline of Philosophy'', in which Russell discussed the [[behaviorism|behaviorist]] philosophy of [[psychologist]] [[John B. Watson]]. At the time, Skinner had begun to take more interest in the actions and behaviors of those around him, and some of his short stories had taken a "psychological" slant. He decided to abandon literature and seek admission as a graduate student in [[psychology]] at [[Harvard University]] (which at the time was not regarded as a leading institution in the field).
  
Skinner received a Ph.D. from Harvard in 1931 and remained at that institution as a researcher until 1936. He then taught at the [[University of Minnesota]] at Minneapolis and later at Indiana University before returning to Harvard as a tenured professor in 1948. He remained there for the rest of his career. In addition to a prolific research record and his influence on a generation of neobehaviorists, Skinner has popularized his behavioristic principles through [[novel]]s and commentaries.
+
Skinner received a Ph.D. from Harvard in 1931, and remained at that institution as a researcher until 1936. He then taught at the [[University of Minnesota]] in Minneapolis, and later at Indiana University, before returning to Harvard as a tenured professor in 1948. He remained there for the rest of his career. In addition to a prolific research record and his influence on a generation of neobehaviorists, Skinner popularized his behavioristic principles through [[novel]]s and commentaries.
  
 
Skinner was granted numerous awards in his lifetime. In 1968, he received the National Medal of Science from President [[Lyndon B. Johnson]]. Three years later, he was awarded the Gold Medal of the American Psychological Foundation, and in 1972, he was given the Humanist of the Year Award of the American Humanist Association. Just eight days before his death, he received the first Citation for Outstanding Lifetime Contribution to Psychology by the American Psychological Association (Epstein, 1997).
 
Skinner was granted numerous awards in his lifetime. In 1968, he received the National Medal of Science from President [[Lyndon B. Johnson]]. Three years later, he was awarded the Gold Medal of the American Psychological Foundation, and in 1972, he was given the Humanist of the Year Award of the American Humanist Association. Just eight days before his death, he received the first Citation for Outstanding Lifetime Contribution to Psychology by the American Psychological Association (Epstein, 1997).
  
Throughout his life, Skinner would seldom attempt to conciliate directly with his critics. Skinner readily argued with them, and sometimes got carried away with clever and acerbic wit, which he later regretted because he might have antagonized his colleagues. However, he also took pride in his forthrightness.
+
Throughout his life, Skinner would seldom attempt to conciliate directly with his critics. He readily argued with them, and sometimes got carried away with clever and acerbic wit, which he later regretted because he might have antagonized his colleagues. However, he also took pride in his forthrightness.
  
 
Skinner's last moments were described in a lovely tribute written by daughter Julie (Vargas, 1990). She wrote of their loving relationship. Admiring his sturdy work ethic, she tried to protect him from it in his last days. He refused final lifesaving efforts, but at the end, when his mouth was dry, Julie noted that "upon receiving a bit of water he said his last word: 'Marvelous.'" Skinner died on August 18, 1990 in Cambridge, Massachussetts.
 
Skinner's last moments were described in a lovely tribute written by daughter Julie (Vargas, 1990). She wrote of their loving relationship. Admiring his sturdy work ethic, she tried to protect him from it in his last days. He refused final lifesaving efforts, but at the end, when his mouth was dry, Julie noted that "upon receiving a bit of water he said his last word: 'Marvelous.'" Skinner died on August 18, 1990 in Cambridge, Massachussetts.
  
=Work=
+
==Work==
 
The basis for B. F. Skinner's research is the study of "operant behavior." In contrast to "respondent behavior," where responses are elicited by specific stimuli, operant behavior is ongoing without any apparent stimulus. To generate operant behavior for investigation, Skinner devised an environmental chamber where [[bird]]s could engage in pecking, or [[rat]]s in bar pressing. In this manner, environmental control was easier to obtain and operant rates could be readily recorded.  
 
The basis for B. F. Skinner's research is the study of "operant behavior." In contrast to "respondent behavior," where responses are elicited by specific stimuli, operant behavior is ongoing without any apparent stimulus. To generate operant behavior for investigation, Skinner devised an environmental chamber where [[bird]]s could engage in pecking, or [[rat]]s in bar pressing. In this manner, environmental control was easier to obtain and operant rates could be readily recorded.  
  
According to Skinner's model, [[learning]] occurs when the ongoing operant comes under the control of reinforcement from the environment. At first, the operant may be shaped by the reinforcement of approximations of the desired operant character. When the refined operant is followed by presentation of the reinforcing event, the probability of the occurance of the operant is increased. For example, if an operant is defined as bar pressing in a rat, presentation of food following a bar press, increases the likelihood of more bar presses. Thus, Skinner's view of reinforcement is defined in terms of the [[probability]] of changes in the operant rate. It avoids inferences of satisfier or annoyer, as in [[Edward L. Thorndike]]'s law of effects, or of drive reduction, as in [[Clark L. Hull]]'s theory.
+
According to Skinner's model, [[learning]] occurs when the ongoing operant comes under the control of reinforcement from the environment. At first, the operant may be shaped by the reinforcement of approximations to the desired operant character. When the refined operant is followed by presentation of the reinforcing event, the probability of the occurance of the operant is increased. For example, if an operant is defined as bar pressing in a rat, presentation of food following a bar press increases the likelihood of more bar presses. Thus, Skinner's view of reinforcement is defined in terms of the [[probability]] of changes in the operant rate. It avoids inferences of a satisfier or annoyer, as in [[Edward L. Thorndike]]'s law of effects, or of drive reduction, as in [[Clark L. Hull]]'s theory.
  
Skinner demonstated the power of reinforcement by showing that characteristic response rates are obtained for particular schedules of reinforcement delivery. Similarly, he translated [[conditioning]] processes such as "generalization" and "discrimination" to a reinforcement contingency framework. Moreover, he extended the principles of operant control to a consideration of verbal behavior. Skinner used those data to argue that behavior is controlled, and the critical role of the [[psychologist]] is to define the parameters of effective control for appropriate social implications.
+
Skinner demonstated the power of reinforcement by showing that characteristic response rates are obtained for particular schedules of reinforcement delivery. Similarly, he translated [[conditioning]] processes such as "generalization" and "discrimination" to a reinforcement contingency framework. Moreover, he extended the principles of operant control to a consideration of verbal behavior. Skinner used his data to argue that behavior is controlled, and the critical role of the [[psychologist]] is to define the parameters of effective control for appropriate social implications.
  
==B. F. Skinner's Neo-behaviorism==
+
===Skinner's Neo-behaviorism===
  
 
{{Main|Behaviorism}}
 
{{Main|Behaviorism}}
Line 37: Line 37:
 
Skinner did not advocate the use of [[punishment]]. His research suggested that punishment was an ineffective way of controlling behavior, leading generally to short-term behavior change, but resulting mostly in the subject attempting to avoid the punishing stimulus, instead of avoiding the behavior that was causing punishment. A simple example of this is the failure of [[prison]] to eliminate [[crime|criminal]] behavior. If prison (as a punishing stimulus) were effective at altering behavior, there would be no criminality, since the risk of imprisonment for criminal conduct is well established. However, individuals still commit offences, but attempt to avoid discovery and therefore punishment. The punishing stimulus does not stop criminal behavior. The criminal simply becomes more sophisticated at avoiding the punishment. Skinner argued that reinforcement, both positive and negative (the latter of which is often confused with punishment), proves to be more effective in bringing about lasting changes in behavior.
 
Skinner did not advocate the use of [[punishment]]. His research suggested that punishment was an ineffective way of controlling behavior, leading generally to short-term behavior change, but resulting mostly in the subject attempting to avoid the punishing stimulus, instead of avoiding the behavior that was causing punishment. A simple example of this is the failure of [[prison]] to eliminate [[crime|criminal]] behavior. If prison (as a punishing stimulus) were effective at altering behavior, there would be no criminality, since the risk of imprisonment for criminal conduct is well established. However, individuals still commit offences, but attempt to avoid discovery and therefore punishment. The punishing stimulus does not stop criminal behavior. The criminal simply becomes more sophisticated at avoiding the punishment. Skinner argued that reinforcement, both positive and negative (the latter of which is often confused with punishment), proves to be more effective in bringing about lasting changes in behavior.
  
==Social engineering==
+
===Superstition among pigeons===
Skinner is popularly known mainly for his controversial books ''Walden Two'' and ''Beyond Freedom and Dignity''. ''Walden Two'' describes a visit to an imaginary [[utopia]]n commune in the United States in the 1940s. In this community, the productivity and happiness of the citizens is far in advance of that in the outside world, due to their practice of scientific social planning and the use of operant conditioning in the raising of children.
 
 
 
''Walden Two'', like [[Henry David Thoreau|Thoreau's]] ''Walden'', champions a lifestyle that does not support [[war]] or foster competition and social strife. It encourages a lifestyle of minimal consumption, rich social relationships, personal happiness, satisfying work, and leisure.
 
 
 
''Beyond Freedom and Dignity'' advanced the thesis that social concepts such as [[free will]] and human dignity (by which Skinner meant belief in individual autonomy) were obsolete, and stood in the way of greater human happiness and productivity. Skinner was opposed to inhumane treatment and bad government, but he argued that the champions of freedom went so far as to deny causality in human action so they could champion the "free person." In a sense, the champions of freedom were enemies of a scientific way of knowing. There is a rough parallel here to the book ''Higher Superstition'' in the identification of opponents to scientific knowledge, except Skinner here is being much more general in the opponents actually identified.
 
 
 
As understood by Skinner, ascribing dignity to individuals involves giving them credit for their actions. To say "Skinner is brilliant" means that Skinner is an originating force. If Skinner is right, he is merely the focus of his environment. He is not an originating force and he had no choice in saying the things he said or doing the things he did. Skinner's environment and [[genetics]] both allowed and compelled him to write his book (though this does not entail that the book's claims are untrue, which is a separate issue). Similarly, the environment and genetic potentials of the advocates of freedom and dignity cause them to resist the reality that their own activities are [[determinism|deterministically]] grounded.
 
 
 
==Superstition among pigeons==
 
 
One of Skinner's experiments examined the formation of "[[superstition]]" in one of his favorite experimental animals, the [[pigeon]]. Skinner placed a series of hungry pigeons in a cage attached to an automatic mechanism that delivered food to the pigeon "at regular intervals with no reference whatsoever to the bird's behavior."  He discovered that the pigeons associated the delivery of the food with whatever chance actions they had been performing as it was delivered, and that they subsequently continued to perform these same actions.
 
One of Skinner's experiments examined the formation of "[[superstition]]" in one of his favorite experimental animals, the [[pigeon]]. Skinner placed a series of hungry pigeons in a cage attached to an automatic mechanism that delivered food to the pigeon "at regular intervals with no reference whatsoever to the bird's behavior."  He discovered that the pigeons associated the delivery of the food with whatever chance actions they had been performing as it was delivered, and that they subsequently continued to perform these same actions.
  
Line 55: Line 46:
 
{{Quotation|The experiment might be said to demonstrate a sort of superstition. The bird behaves as if there were a causal relation between its behavior and the presentation of food, although such a relation is lacking. There are many analogies in human behavior. Rituals for changing one's fortune at cards are good examples. A few accidental connections between a ritual and favorable consequences suffice to set up and maintain the behavior in spite of many unreinforced instances. The bowler who has released a ball down the alley but continues to behave as if she were controlling it by twisting and turning her arm and shoulder is another case in point. These behaviors have, of course, no real effect upon one's luck or upon a ball half way down an alley, just as in the present case the food would appear as often if the pigeon did nothing—or, more strictly speaking, did something else.|B. F. Skinner|"'Superstition' in the Pigeon", ''Journal of Experimental Psychology'' #38, 1947 [http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Skinner/Pigeon/]}}
 
{{Quotation|The experiment might be said to demonstrate a sort of superstition. The bird behaves as if there were a causal relation between its behavior and the presentation of food, although such a relation is lacking. There are many analogies in human behavior. Rituals for changing one's fortune at cards are good examples. A few accidental connections between a ritual and favorable consequences suffice to set up and maintain the behavior in spite of many unreinforced instances. The bowler who has released a ball down the alley but continues to behave as if she were controlling it by twisting and turning her arm and shoulder is another case in point. These behaviors have, of course, no real effect upon one's luck or upon a ball half way down an alley, just as in the present case the food would appear as often if the pigeon did nothing—or, more strictly speaking, did something else.|B. F. Skinner|"'Superstition' in the Pigeon", ''Journal of Experimental Psychology'' #38, 1947 [http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Skinner/Pigeon/]}}
  
Modern behavioral psychologists have disputed Skinner's "superstition" explanation for the behaviors he recorded. Subsequent research (for instance, by Staddon and Simmelhag in 1971) failed to replicate his results. Eduardo J. Fernandez of the Department of Psychology of Indiana University sought to follow up on Staddon and Simmelhag's debunking of Skinner's hypothesis and to "further contrast superstitious versus functional interpretations of behavior" in pigeons. In a 2004 paper titled "Superstition Re-revisited: An Examination of Niche-Related Mechanisms Underlying Schedule Produced Behavior in Pigeons," he demonstrated that what Skinner had seen as "superstitious" behaviour was accounted for by the natural foraging behaviors of the species he used as test subjects.
+
Modern behavioral psychologists have disputed Skinner's "superstition" explanation for the behaviors he recorded. Subsequent research (for instance, by Staddon and Simmelhag in 1971) failed to replicate his results. Eduardo J. Fernandez of the Department of Psychology of Indiana University sought to follow up on Staddon and Simmelhag's debunking of Skinner's hypothesis and to "further contrast superstitious versus functional interpretations of behavior" in pigeons. In a 2004 paper titled "Superstition Re-revisited: An Examination of Niche-Related Mechanisms Underlying Schedule Produced Behavior in Pigeons," he demonstrated that what Skinner had seen as "superstitious" behavior was accounted for by the natural foraging behaviors of the species he used as test subjects.
  
==Political views==
+
===Social engineering===
 +
Skinner is popularly known for his controversial books ''Walden Two'' and ''Beyond Freedom and Dignity''. ''Walden Two'' describes a visit to an imaginary [[utopia]]n commune in the United States in the 1940s. In this community, the productivity and happiness of the citizens is far in advance of that in the outside world, due to their practice of scientific social planning and the use of operant conditioning in the raising of children.
 +
 
 +
''Walden Two'', like [[Henry David Thoreau|Thoreau's]] ''Walden'', champions a lifestyle that does not support [[war]] or foster competition and social strife. It encourages a lifestyle of minimal consumption, rich social relationships, personal happiness, satisfying work, and leisure.
 +
 
 +
''Beyond Freedom and Dignity'' advanced the thesis that social concepts such as [[free will]] and human dignity (by which Skinner meant belief in individual autonomy) were obsolete, and stood in the way of greater human happiness and productivity. Skinner was opposed to inhumane treatment and bad government, but he argued that the champions of freedom went so far as to deny causality in human action so they could champion the "free person." In a sense, the champions of freedom were enemies of the scientific way of knowing. And, Skinner believed, this freedom they sought might well lead to their self-destruction. For Skinner, it was not freedom that lit the way to happiness, but rather science held the keys to true human fulfillment.
 +
 
 +
===Political views===
 
Skinner's political writings emphasized his hopes that an effective and humane science of behavioral control—a behavioral technology—could solve human problems, which had not been solved by earlier approaches or were actively aggravated by advances in physical [[technology]] such as the [[atomic bomb]]. One of Skinner's stated goals was to prevent humanity from destroying itself.
 
Skinner's political writings emphasized his hopes that an effective and humane science of behavioral control—a behavioral technology—could solve human problems, which had not been solved by earlier approaches or were actively aggravated by advances in physical [[technology]] such as the [[atomic bomb]]. One of Skinner's stated goals was to prevent humanity from destroying itself.
  
Skinner was sometimes accused of being a [[totalitarian]] by his critics. Intellectual opponents, ranging from [[Noam Chomsky]] to [[Ayn Rand]], in their attempt to show Skinner wrong, equated Skinner's philosophic determinism with political oppression. Skinner has often been equated to political and social positions he never espoused and even explicitly objected to.
+
Skinner was sometimes accused of being a [[totalitarian]] by his critics. Intellectual opponents, ranging from [[Noam Chomsky]] to [[Ayn Rand]], in their attempt to show Skinner wrong, equated his philosophic determinism with political oppression. Skinner has often been equated to political and social positions he never espoused and even explicitly objected to.
  
Skinner was a [[determinism|determinist]], believing that all of our behavior is profoundly determined and influenced by the environment. Skinner saw the problems of political control not as a battle of domination versus freedom, but as choices of what kinds of control were used for what purposes. Skinner opposed the use of coercion, [[punishment]], and fear, supporting the use of positive reinforcement or reward.
+
Skinner was a [[determinism|determinist]], believing that all of our behavior is profoundly determined and influenced by the environment. He saw the problems of political control not as a battle of domination versus freedom, but as choices of what kinds of control were used for what purposes. Skinner opposed the use of coercion, [[punishment]], and fear, supporting the use of positive reinforcement or reward.
  
Skinner's book ''Walden Two'' presents a vision of a decentralized, localized society which applies a practical, scientific approach and futuristically advanced behavioral expertise to peacefully deal with social problems. Skinner's [[utopia]], like every other utopia or [[dystopia]], is both a [[thought]] experiment and a [[rhetoric]]al work. In it, Skinner answers a problem that exists in many utopian novels: "What is the Good Life?" Skinner answers that it is a life of friendship, health, art, a healthy balance between work and leisure, a minimum of unpleasantness, and a feeling that one has made worthwhile contributions to one's society.  
+
Skinner's book ''Walden Two'' presents a vision of a decentralized, localized society which applies a practical, scientific approach and futuristically advanced behavioral expertise to peacefully deal with social problems. His [[utopia]], like every other utopia or [[dystopia]], is both a [[thought]] experiment and a [[rhetoric]]al work. In it, he answers a problem that exists in many utopian novels: "What is the Good Life?" Skinner answers that it is a life of friendship, health, art, a healthy balance between work and leisure, a minimum of unpleasantness, and a feeling that one has made worthwhile contributions to one's society.  
  
Additionally, Skinner believed that behavioral technology would offer alternatives to coercion, good science applied right would help society, and we would all be better off if we cooperated with each other peacefully. Skinner's novel was described by Skinner as "my New Atlantis" referring to [[Francis Bacon|Bacon]]'s utopia.  
+
Additionally, Skinner believed that behavioral technology would offer alternatives to coercion, good science applied right would help society, and we would all be better off if we cooperated with each other peacefully.  
  
 
==Criticism==
 
==Criticism==
B. F. Skinner's view of behavior has drawn the often harsh criticism of many who are offended by his mechanical conception of human nature. Moreover, whether one considers Skinner's environmental determinacy or [[Pavlov]]'s physiological reductionism, the net conceptualization of human activity precludes any attributes of personal freedom, self-determinacy, or the dynamics of [[consciousness]]. Skinner has earned more of the scorn of critics because he has articulated the social controls that are derived from the principles of operant behavior.
+
B. F. Skinner's view of behavior has drawn the often harsh criticism of many who are offended by his mechanical conception of human nature. Moreover, whether one considers Skinner's environmental determinacy or [[Pavlov]]'s physiological reductionism, the net conceptualization of human activity precludes any attributes of personal freedom, self-determinacy, or the dynamics of [[consciousness]]. Skinner earned more of the scorn of critics because he articulated the social controls that are derived from the principles of operant behavior.
  
As are most people with grand ideas, strong convictions, and tenacious efforts to enlarge their influence, B. F. Skinner was constantly attacked by adversaries critisizing most of his views and attempting to establish in every possible way that he is fatally flawed. Although [[Sigmund Freud]] and Skinner were opposites on the importance of [[introspection]], Skinner used to consider the two of them fundamental allies in propagandizing for the primacy of environmental influences on human behavior.
+
As are most people with grand ideas, strong convictions, and tenacious efforts to enlarge their influence, B. F. Skinner was constantly attacked by adversaries criticizing his views, and attempting to establish in every possible way that his ideas were fatally flawed. Although [[Sigmund Freud]] and Skinner were opposites on the importance of [[introspection]], Skinner used to consider the two of them fundamental allies in propagandizing for the primacy of environmental influences on human behavior.
  
 
==Rumors==
 
==Rumors==
Line 78: Line 76:
 
In fact, the "Heir Conditioner," a term for Skinner's baby crib, was heated, cooled, had filtered air, allowed plenty of space to walk around in, and was much like a miniature version of a modern home. It was designed to make the baby more confident, more comfortable, less sick, less prone to cry, and so on. Reportedly it had some success in these goals.<ref name=snopes>[http://www.snopes.com/science/skinner.asp Snopes article on the "Skinner box"]</ref>
 
In fact, the "Heir Conditioner," a term for Skinner's baby crib, was heated, cooled, had filtered air, allowed plenty of space to walk around in, and was much like a miniature version of a modern home. It was designed to make the baby more confident, more comfortable, less sick, less prone to cry, and so on. Reportedly it had some success in these goals.<ref name=snopes>[http://www.snopes.com/science/skinner.asp Snopes article on the "Skinner box"]</ref>
  
In 2004, psychologist and author [[Lauren Slater]] published a book, ''Opening Skinner's Box'', which mentioned claims that Deborah Skinner (now Deborah Skinner Buzan) unsuccessfully sued her father for abuse, and later committed [[suicide]]. In response, Buzan herself came forward to publicly denounce the story as nothing more than hearsay and presumably to vouch for her own continued existence. She blasted Lauren Slater's book for repeating this urban legend as being vicious and harmful. <ref name =guardian>[http://books.guardian.co.uk/departments/healthmindandbody/story/0,6000,1168052,00.html Guardian Newspapers Limited - online]</ref> [[Farhad Manjoo]], a writer for [[Salon.com]], protested in a 2004 literary review that Buzan's Guardian article 'reads as if she has never even picked up Slater's book', observed that 'Slater's description of the box is pretty much in line with Buzan's description in the Guardian', and called the book 'a genuinely compelling read'. <ref name=Manjoo>>[http://www.powells.com/review/2004_05_28.html ''How free is free will?'' Salon.com (on Powell Books) by Farhad Manjoo, 28 May 2004]. Manjoo continued, 'Slater writes that it was actually an "an upgraded playpen" whose "thermostatically controlled environment" prevented diaper rash and other kiddie ailments, reduced the chance of suffocation by blanket, and allowed the daughter to walk around without any impediments, building a baby of impressive self-confidence.'</ref>
+
In 2004, psychologist and author Lauren Slater published a book, ''Opening Skinner's Box'', which mentioned claims that Deborah Skinner (now Deborah Skinner Buzan) unsuccessfully sued her father for abuse, and later committed [[suicide]]. In response, Buzan herself came forward to publicly denounce the story as nothing more than hearsay and presumably to vouch for her own continued existence. She blasted Lauren Slater's book for repeating this urban legend as being vicious and harmful. <ref name =guardian>[http://books.guardian.co.uk/departments/healthmindandbody/story/0,6000,1168052,00.html I was not a lab rat], response by Skinner's daughter about the "baby box" in Guardian Newspapers Limited online.</ref>  
  
=Skinner's Legacy: Radical Positivism=
+
==Legacy: Radical Positivism==
Skinner's [[positivism]] has consistently advocated a methodological emphasis and return to the study of behavior defined in terms of peripheral events. Skinner argued against speculating about central mediating agencies of behavior, whether [[cognitive]] or physiological. Rather, behavior for Skinner is completely subject to environmental determinacy. If the envorinment is controlled, behavior is controlled. For this reason, Skinner accepts the validity of exhaustive study of a single subject, because variability arises not from individual differences inherent in the organism but from differential environmental contingencies.
+
Skinner's [[positivism]] has consistently advocated a methodological emphasis and return to the study of behavior defined in terms of peripheral events. Skinner argued against speculating about central mediating agencies of behavior, whether [[cognitive]] or [[physiology|physiological]]. Rather, behavior for Skinner is completely subject to environmental determinacy. If the envoronment is controlled, behavior is controlled. For this reason, Skinner accepted the validity of exhaustive study of a single subject, because variability arises not from individual differences inherent in the organism but from differential environmental contingencies.
  
 
In the spirit of positivism, Skinner argued that the so-called humanistic characteristics of species, which presumably set us off from the rest of living evolutionary products, are in fact an illusion, created by us over history to give us sense of security. In fact, for Skinner to be human means to be in control, to understand and use environmental contingencies to self benefit.
 
In the spirit of positivism, Skinner argued that the so-called humanistic characteristics of species, which presumably set us off from the rest of living evolutionary products, are in fact an illusion, created by us over history to give us sense of security. In fact, for Skinner to be human means to be in control, to understand and use environmental contingencies to self benefit.
  
 
A natural outgrowth of [[behaviorism]] is [[behavior therapy]], a technique of altering an individual's maladaptive reactions to particular stimuli. It involves the most basic of methods to alter human behavior, such as reward and punishment, reinforcement, and even [[biofeedback]], using [[conditioning]] techniques. The cultivation of life skills is often a central focus. While founded in behaviorism, such forms of behavior modification are used by psychotherapists, parents, and caretakers of the disabled, generally without any underlying behaviorist philosophy.
 
A natural outgrowth of [[behaviorism]] is [[behavior therapy]], a technique of altering an individual's maladaptive reactions to particular stimuli. It involves the most basic of methods to alter human behavior, such as reward and punishment, reinforcement, and even [[biofeedback]], using [[conditioning]] techniques. The cultivation of life skills is often a central focus. While founded in behaviorism, such forms of behavior modification are used by psychotherapists, parents, and caretakers of the disabled, generally without any underlying behaviorist philosophy.
 +
 +
Skinner's work has also been applied to the field of [[education]]. He formulated principles of [[programmed learning]], in which reinforcement of small, incremental steps with immediate reinforcement, or reward, for the correct responses would presumably lead to learning not only of sensorimotor responses but also of verbal responses and conceptual knowledge. In fact, his ideas have been successfully incorporated in "teaching machines" as well as [[computer assisted instruction]].
  
 
==Publications==
 
==Publications==
Line 108: Line 108:
 
*''Recent Issues in the Analysis of Behavior'', 1989. <small>ISBN 0-675-20674-X.</small>
 
*''Recent Issues in the Analysis of Behavior'', 1989. <small>ISBN 0-675-20674-X.</small>
 
*''Cumulative Record: A Selection of Papers'', 1959, 1961, 1972 and 1999 as ''Cumulative Record: Definitive Edition''. This book includes the authentic account of the much-misrepresented "Baby in a box" device. <small>ISBN 1-58390-00505.</small>
 
*''Cumulative Record: A Selection of Papers'', 1959, 1961, 1972 and 1999 as ''Cumulative Record: Definitive Edition''. This book includes the authentic account of the much-misrepresented "Baby in a box" device. <small>ISBN 1-58390-00505.</small>
 +
 +
===Articles by Skinner===
 +
*[http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Skinner/Twotypes/twotypes.htm Two Types of Conditioned Reflex and a Pseudo Type (1935)] 
 +
*[http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Skinner/Pigeon/ "Superstition" in the Pigeon (1947)] 
 +
*[http://psychclassics.asu.edu/Skinner/Theories/ Are Theories of Learning Necessary? (1950)]
 +
 +
==Notes==
 +
<references/>
  
 
==References==
 
==References==
* Brennan, J.F.(1982). '''History and systems of psychology'''. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
+
* Brennan, J.F. 1982. ''History and systems of psychology''. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
* Epstein, R. (1997) Skinner as self-manager. ''Journal of applied behavior analysis''. 30, 545-569. Retrieved from the world wide web on: June 2, 2005 from http://seab.envmed.rochester.edu/jaba/articles/1997/jaba-30-03-0545.pdf  
+
* Epstein, R. 1997 [http://seab.envmed.rochester.edu/jaba/articles/1997/jaba-30-03-0545.pdf "Skinner as self-manager"] ''Journal of applied behavior analysis''. 30, 545-569.
<references/>
+
* Skinner B. F. 1976. ''Particulars of my life''. New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.
* Skinner B. F. (1976). '''Particulars of my life'''. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
+
* Slater, Lauren. 2004. ''Opening Skinner's Box: Great Psychological Experiments Of The 20th Century''. Bill Daniels Co. ISBN 0747563179
*Vargas, J. S. (1990, Winter). B. F. Skinner: The last few days. ''Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis'', 409-410.
+
* Vargas, J. S. 1990. B. F. Skinner: The last few days. ''Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis'', 409-410. Winter, 1990.
* Wiener, D. N. (1996). B. F. '''Skinner: Benign anarchist'''. Boston et.al.:Allyn and Bacon.
+
* Wiener, D. N. 1996. ''B. F. Skinner: Benign anarchist''. Boston, MA:Allyn and Bacon.
  
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
Line 124: Line 132:
 
*[http://www.nndb.com/people/297/000022231/ B.F. Skinner profile, NNDB].
 
*[http://www.nndb.com/people/297/000022231/ B.F. Skinner profile, NNDB].
 
*[http://www.loshorcones.org.mx/index-eng.php/ Los Horcones Walden Two Community].
 
*[http://www.loshorcones.org.mx/index-eng.php/ Los Horcones Walden Two Community].
*[http://books.guardian.co.uk/departments/healthmindandbody/story/0,6000,1168052,00.html I was not a lab rat], response by Skinner's daughter about the "baby box". 
 
 
''Articles by Skinner:'' 
 
*[http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Skinner/Twotypes/twotypes.htm Two Types of Conditioned Reflex and a Pseudo Type (1935)] 
 
*[http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Skinner/Pigeon/ "Superstition" in the Pigeon (1947)] 
 
*[http://psychclassics.asu.edu/Skinner/Theories/ Are Theories of Learning Necessary? (1950)]
 
  
 
{{Credit1|Burrhus_Frederic_Skinner|67941081|}}
 
{{Credit1|Burrhus_Frederic_Skinner|67941081|}}

Revision as of 16:49, 18 October 2006



File:Skinner.jpg
Burrhus Frederic Skinner

Burrhus Frederic Skinner (March 20, 1904 – August 18, 1990) was an American psychologist and author. He conducted pioneering work in experimental psychology and advocated behaviorism, which seeks to understand and control behavior as a function of environmental histories of reinforcement. Skinner alos wrote a number of controversial works in which he proposed the widespread use of psychological behavior modification techniques, primarily using conditioning, in order to improve society and increase human happiness.

Life

Burrhus Frederic Skinner was born in rural Susquehanna, Pennsylvania. He attended Hamilton College in New York, with the intention of becoming a writer and received a B.A. in English literature in 1926.

After graduation, he spent a year in Greenwich Village attempting to become a writer of fiction, but he soon became disillusioned with his literary skills. He concluded that he had little world experience and no strong personal perspective from which to write. During this time, which Skinner later called "the dark year," he chanced upon a copy of Bertrand Russell's recently published book An Outline of Philosophy, in which Russell discussed the behaviorist philosophy of psychologist John B. Watson. At the time, Skinner had begun to take more interest in the actions and behaviors of those around him, and some of his short stories had taken a "psychological" slant. He decided to abandon literature and seek admission as a graduate student in psychology at Harvard University (which at the time was not regarded as a leading institution in the field).

Skinner received a Ph.D. from Harvard in 1931, and remained at that institution as a researcher until 1936. He then taught at the University of Minnesota in Minneapolis, and later at Indiana University, before returning to Harvard as a tenured professor in 1948. He remained there for the rest of his career. In addition to a prolific research record and his influence on a generation of neobehaviorists, Skinner popularized his behavioristic principles through novels and commentaries.

Skinner was granted numerous awards in his lifetime. In 1968, he received the National Medal of Science from President Lyndon B. Johnson. Three years later, he was awarded the Gold Medal of the American Psychological Foundation, and in 1972, he was given the Humanist of the Year Award of the American Humanist Association. Just eight days before his death, he received the first Citation for Outstanding Lifetime Contribution to Psychology by the American Psychological Association (Epstein, 1997).

Throughout his life, Skinner would seldom attempt to conciliate directly with his critics. He readily argued with them, and sometimes got carried away with clever and acerbic wit, which he later regretted because he might have antagonized his colleagues. However, he also took pride in his forthrightness.

Skinner's last moments were described in a lovely tribute written by daughter Julie (Vargas, 1990). She wrote of their loving relationship. Admiring his sturdy work ethic, she tried to protect him from it in his last days. He refused final lifesaving efforts, but at the end, when his mouth was dry, Julie noted that "upon receiving a bit of water he said his last word: 'Marvelous.'" Skinner died on August 18, 1990 in Cambridge, Massachussetts.

Work

The basis for B. F. Skinner's research is the study of "operant behavior." In contrast to "respondent behavior," where responses are elicited by specific stimuli, operant behavior is ongoing without any apparent stimulus. To generate operant behavior for investigation, Skinner devised an environmental chamber where birds could engage in pecking, or rats in bar pressing. In this manner, environmental control was easier to obtain and operant rates could be readily recorded.

According to Skinner's model, learning occurs when the ongoing operant comes under the control of reinforcement from the environment. At first, the operant may be shaped by the reinforcement of approximations to the desired operant character. When the refined operant is followed by presentation of the reinforcing event, the probability of the occurance of the operant is increased. For example, if an operant is defined as bar pressing in a rat, presentation of food following a bar press increases the likelihood of more bar presses. Thus, Skinner's view of reinforcement is defined in terms of the probability of changes in the operant rate. It avoids inferences of a satisfier or annoyer, as in Edward L. Thorndike's law of effects, or of drive reduction, as in Clark L. Hull's theory.

Skinner demonstated the power of reinforcement by showing that characteristic response rates are obtained for particular schedules of reinforcement delivery. Similarly, he translated conditioning processes such as "generalization" and "discrimination" to a reinforcement contingency framework. Moreover, he extended the principles of operant control to a consideration of verbal behavior. Skinner used his data to argue that behavior is controlled, and the critical role of the psychologist is to define the parameters of effective control for appropriate social implications.

Skinner's Neo-behaviorism

Main article: Behaviorism

Skinner was mainly responsible for the development of the philosophy of radical behaviorism and for the further development of applied behavior analysis, a branch of psychology which aims to develop a unified framework for animal and human behavior based on principles of learning. He conducted research on shaping behavior through positive and negative reinforcement, and demonstrated operant conditioning, a behavior modification technique which he developed in contrast with Ivan Pavlov's classical conditioning.

Skinner did not advocate the use of punishment. His research suggested that punishment was an ineffective way of controlling behavior, leading generally to short-term behavior change, but resulting mostly in the subject attempting to avoid the punishing stimulus, instead of avoiding the behavior that was causing punishment. A simple example of this is the failure of prison to eliminate criminal behavior. If prison (as a punishing stimulus) were effective at altering behavior, there would be no criminality, since the risk of imprisonment for criminal conduct is well established. However, individuals still commit offences, but attempt to avoid discovery and therefore punishment. The punishing stimulus does not stop criminal behavior. The criminal simply becomes more sophisticated at avoiding the punishment. Skinner argued that reinforcement, both positive and negative (the latter of which is often confused with punishment), proves to be more effective in bringing about lasting changes in behavior.

Superstition among pigeons

One of Skinner's experiments examined the formation of "superstition" in one of his favorite experimental animals, the pigeon. Skinner placed a series of hungry pigeons in a cage attached to an automatic mechanism that delivered food to the pigeon "at regular intervals with no reference whatsoever to the bird's behavior." He discovered that the pigeons associated the delivery of the food with whatever chance actions they had been performing as it was delivered, and that they subsequently continued to perform these same actions.

One bird was conditioned to turn counter-clockwise about the cage, making two or three turns between reinforcements. Another repeatedly thrust its head into one of the upper corners of the cage. A third developed a 'tossing' response, as if placing its head beneath an invisible bar and lifting it repeatedly. Two birds developed a pendulum motion of the head and body, in which the head was extended forward and swung from right to left with a sharp movement followed by a somewhat slower return.

B. F. Skinner, "'Superstition' in the Pigeon", Journal of Experimental Psychology #38, 1947 [1]

Skinner suggested that the pigeons believed that they were influencing the automatic mechanism with their "rituals" and that the experiment shed light on human behavior:

The experiment might be said to demonstrate a sort of superstition. The bird behaves as if there were a causal relation between its behavior and the presentation of food, although such a relation is lacking. There are many analogies in human behavior. Rituals for changing one's fortune at cards are good examples. A few accidental connections between a ritual and favorable consequences suffice to set up and maintain the behavior in spite of many unreinforced instances. The bowler who has released a ball down the alley but continues to behave as if she were controlling it by twisting and turning her arm and shoulder is another case in point. These behaviors have, of course, no real effect upon one's luck or upon a ball half way down an alley, just as in the present case the food would appear as often if the pigeon did nothing—or, more strictly speaking, did something else.

B. F. Skinner, "'Superstition' in the Pigeon", Journal of Experimental Psychology #38, 1947 [2]

Modern behavioral psychologists have disputed Skinner's "superstition" explanation for the behaviors he recorded. Subsequent research (for instance, by Staddon and Simmelhag in 1971) failed to replicate his results. Eduardo J. Fernandez of the Department of Psychology of Indiana University sought to follow up on Staddon and Simmelhag's debunking of Skinner's hypothesis and to "further contrast superstitious versus functional interpretations of behavior" in pigeons. In a 2004 paper titled "Superstition Re-revisited: An Examination of Niche-Related Mechanisms Underlying Schedule Produced Behavior in Pigeons," he demonstrated that what Skinner had seen as "superstitious" behavior was accounted for by the natural foraging behaviors of the species he used as test subjects.

Social engineering

Skinner is popularly known for his controversial books Walden Two and Beyond Freedom and Dignity. Walden Two describes a visit to an imaginary utopian commune in the United States in the 1940s. In this community, the productivity and happiness of the citizens is far in advance of that in the outside world, due to their practice of scientific social planning and the use of operant conditioning in the raising of children.

Walden Two, like Thoreau's Walden, champions a lifestyle that does not support war or foster competition and social strife. It encourages a lifestyle of minimal consumption, rich social relationships, personal happiness, satisfying work, and leisure.

Beyond Freedom and Dignity advanced the thesis that social concepts such as free will and human dignity (by which Skinner meant belief in individual autonomy) were obsolete, and stood in the way of greater human happiness and productivity. Skinner was opposed to inhumane treatment and bad government, but he argued that the champions of freedom went so far as to deny causality in human action so they could champion the "free person." In a sense, the champions of freedom were enemies of the scientific way of knowing. And, Skinner believed, this freedom they sought might well lead to their self-destruction. For Skinner, it was not freedom that lit the way to happiness, but rather science held the keys to true human fulfillment.

Political views

Skinner's political writings emphasized his hopes that an effective and humane science of behavioral control—a behavioral technology—could solve human problems, which had not been solved by earlier approaches or were actively aggravated by advances in physical technology such as the atomic bomb. One of Skinner's stated goals was to prevent humanity from destroying itself.

Skinner was sometimes accused of being a totalitarian by his critics. Intellectual opponents, ranging from Noam Chomsky to Ayn Rand, in their attempt to show Skinner wrong, equated his philosophic determinism with political oppression. Skinner has often been equated to political and social positions he never espoused and even explicitly objected to.

Skinner was a determinist, believing that all of our behavior is profoundly determined and influenced by the environment. He saw the problems of political control not as a battle of domination versus freedom, but as choices of what kinds of control were used for what purposes. Skinner opposed the use of coercion, punishment, and fear, supporting the use of positive reinforcement or reward.

Skinner's book Walden Two presents a vision of a decentralized, localized society which applies a practical, scientific approach and futuristically advanced behavioral expertise to peacefully deal with social problems. His utopia, like every other utopia or dystopia, is both a thought experiment and a rhetorical work. In it, he answers a problem that exists in many utopian novels: "What is the Good Life?" Skinner answers that it is a life of friendship, health, art, a healthy balance between work and leisure, a minimum of unpleasantness, and a feeling that one has made worthwhile contributions to one's society.

Additionally, Skinner believed that behavioral technology would offer alternatives to coercion, good science applied right would help society, and we would all be better off if we cooperated with each other peacefully.

Criticism

B. F. Skinner's view of behavior has drawn the often harsh criticism of many who are offended by his mechanical conception of human nature. Moreover, whether one considers Skinner's environmental determinacy or Pavlov's physiological reductionism, the net conceptualization of human activity precludes any attributes of personal freedom, self-determinacy, or the dynamics of consciousness. Skinner earned more of the scorn of critics because he articulated the social controls that are derived from the principles of operant behavior.

As are most people with grand ideas, strong convictions, and tenacious efforts to enlarge their influence, B. F. Skinner was constantly attacked by adversaries criticizing his views, and attempting to establish in every possible way that his ideas were fatally flawed. Although Sigmund Freud and Skinner were opposites on the importance of introspection, Skinner used to consider the two of them fundamental allies in propagandizing for the primacy of environmental influences on human behavior.

Rumors

One often-repeated story claims that Skinner ventured into human experiments by raising his daughter Deborah in a "Skinner box," which led to her life-long mental illness and a bitter resentment towards her father.

In fact, the "Heir Conditioner," a term for Skinner's baby crib, was heated, cooled, had filtered air, allowed plenty of space to walk around in, and was much like a miniature version of a modern home. It was designed to make the baby more confident, more comfortable, less sick, less prone to cry, and so on. Reportedly it had some success in these goals.[1]

In 2004, psychologist and author Lauren Slater published a book, Opening Skinner's Box, which mentioned claims that Deborah Skinner (now Deborah Skinner Buzan) unsuccessfully sued her father for abuse, and later committed suicide. In response, Buzan herself came forward to publicly denounce the story as nothing more than hearsay and presumably to vouch for her own continued existence. She blasted Lauren Slater's book for repeating this urban legend as being vicious and harmful. [2]

Legacy: Radical Positivism

Skinner's positivism has consistently advocated a methodological emphasis and return to the study of behavior defined in terms of peripheral events. Skinner argued against speculating about central mediating agencies of behavior, whether cognitive or physiological. Rather, behavior for Skinner is completely subject to environmental determinacy. If the envoronment is controlled, behavior is controlled. For this reason, Skinner accepted the validity of exhaustive study of a single subject, because variability arises not from individual differences inherent in the organism but from differential environmental contingencies.

In the spirit of positivism, Skinner argued that the so-called humanistic characteristics of species, which presumably set us off from the rest of living evolutionary products, are in fact an illusion, created by us over history to give us sense of security. In fact, for Skinner to be human means to be in control, to understand and use environmental contingencies to self benefit.

A natural outgrowth of behaviorism is behavior therapy, a technique of altering an individual's maladaptive reactions to particular stimuli. It involves the most basic of methods to alter human behavior, such as reward and punishment, reinforcement, and even biofeedback, using conditioning techniques. The cultivation of life skills is often a central focus. While founded in behaviorism, such forms of behavior modification are used by psychotherapists, parents, and caretakers of the disabled, generally without any underlying behaviorist philosophy.

Skinner's work has also been applied to the field of education. He formulated principles of programmed learning, in which reinforcement of small, incremental steps with immediate reinforcement, or reward, for the correct responses would presumably lead to learning not only of sensorimotor responses but also of verbal responses and conceptual knowledge. In fact, his ideas have been successfully incorporated in "teaching machines" as well as computer assisted instruction.

Publications

  • The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis, 1938. ISBN 1-58390-007-1, ISBN 0-87411-487-X.
  • Walden Two, 1948. ISBN 0-02-411510-X.
  • Science and Human Behavior, 1953. ISBN 0-02-929040-6.
  • Schedules of Reinforcement, with C. B. Ferster, 1957. ISBN 0-13-792309-0.
  • Verbal Behavior, 1957. ISBN 1-58390-021-7.
  • The Analysis of Behavior: A Program for Self Instruction, with James G. Holland, 1961. This self-instruction book is no longer in print, but the B.F. Skinner Foundation web site has an interactive version. ISBN 07-029565-4.
  • The Technology of Teaching, 1968.
  • Contingencies of Reinforcement: A Theoretical Analysis, 1969. ISBN 390-81280-3.
  • Beyond Freedom and Dignity, 1971. ISBN 0-394-42555-3.
  • About Behaviorism, 1974. ISBN 0-394-49201-3
  • Particulars of My Life: Part One of an Autobiography, 1976. ISBN 0-394-40071-2.
  • Reflections on Behaviorism and Society, 1978. ISBN 0-13-770057-1.
  • The Shaping of a Behaviorist: Part Two of an Autobiography, 1979. ISBN 0-394-50581-6.
  • Notebooks, edited by Robert Epstein, 1980. ISBN 0-13-624106-9.
  • Skinner for the Classroom, edited by R. Epstein, 1982. ISBN 0-87822-261-8.
  • Enjoy Old Age: A Program of Self-Management, with M. E. Vaughan, 1983.
  • A Matter of Consequences: Part Three of an Autobiography, 1983. ISBN 0-394-53266-0, ISBN 0-8147-7845-3.
  • Upon Further Reflection, 1987. ISBN 0-13-938986-5.
  • Recent Issues in the Analysis of Behavior, 1989. ISBN 0-675-20674-X.
  • Cumulative Record: A Selection of Papers, 1959, 1961, 1972 and 1999 as Cumulative Record: Definitive Edition. This book includes the authentic account of the much-misrepresented "Baby in a box" device. ISBN 1-58390-00505.

Articles by Skinner

Notes

  1. Snopes article on the "Skinner box"
  2. I was not a lab rat, response by Skinner's daughter about the "baby box" in Guardian Newspapers Limited online.

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Brennan, J.F. 1982. History and systems of psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
  • Epstein, R. 1997 "Skinner as self-manager" Journal of applied behavior analysis. 30, 545-569.
  • Skinner B. F. 1976. Particulars of my life. New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.
  • Slater, Lauren. 2004. Opening Skinner's Box: Great Psychological Experiments Of The 20th Century. Bill Daniels Co. ISBN 0747563179
  • Vargas, J. S. 1990. B. F. Skinner: The last few days. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 409-410. Winter, 1990.
  • Wiener, D. N. 1996. B. F. Skinner: Benign anarchist. Boston, MA:Allyn and Bacon.

External links

Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.