Sapir, Edward

From New World Encyclopedia
(copied from Wikipedia)
 
 
(24 intermediate revisions by 7 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
[[Category:Anthropology]]
+
[[Category:Anthropologists]]
[[Category:Linguistics]]
+
[[Category:Linguists and lexicographers]]
[[Category:Biography]]
 
{{epname}}
 
  
 +
{{epname|Sapir, Edward}}
 +
{{Paid}}{{Approved}}{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}{{copyedited}}
 +
'''Edward Sapir''' (January 26, 1884 – February 4, 1939) was an [[United States|American]] [[anthropology|anthropologist]]-[[linguistics|linguist]], a leader in American [[structuralism|structural linguistics]]. He is arguably the most influential figure in American linguistics. His anthropological approach, influenced by [[Franz Boas]], began with his research on [[Native American]]s in an attempt to document their languages before they disappeared. This led to his interest in the inter-connections among [[language]], [[thinking]], and [[culture]]. Work with [[Benjamin Whorf]] in this area led to what became known as the "[[Sapir-Whorf hypothesis]]" and the development of the field of [[ethnolinguistics]]. Sapir's belief that language shapes [[perception]] and influences behavior led him to research the nature of an international auxiliary language. In order to function as a harmonious global society, effective [[communication]] must be achieved, and Sapir's work was instrumental in developing the understanding of how people may communicate without misunderstanding.
 +
{{toc}}
 +
==Life==
  
'''Edward Sapir''' ([[IPA chart for English|pronounced]] {{IPA|/səˈpiɹ/}}), ([[January 26]] [[1884]] – [[February 4]] [[1939]]) was an [[United States|American]] [[anthropologist]]-[[linguist]], a leader in American [[structuralism|structural linguistics]], and one of the creators of what is now called the [[Sapir-Whorf hypothesis]]. He is arguably the most influential figure in American linguistics, influencing even [[Noam Chomsky]].
+
Edward Sapir was born on January 26, 1884 in Lauenburg, [[Prussia]], (now [[Lębork]] in [[Poland]]) to an orthodox [[Judaism|Jewish]] family of [[Lithuania]]n origins. His parents, Jacob David and Eva Seagal Sapir spoke [[Yiddish]] amongst themselves, so Edward learned both German and the language of his parents. When he was six years old his family immigrated to the [[United States]], and eventually settled on the Lower East Side of [[New York City]]. After the death of his younger brother Max, and numerous problems, Sapir’s parents [[divorce]]d in 1910.
  
==Life and work==
+
Sapir entered [[Columbia University]] in 1901, on a Pulitzer scholarship he had won three years earlier. He was an extremely bright young man, and many professors saw great potential in him. At Columbia he studied German [[philology]] and Indo-European [[linguistics]]. In 1904, he graduated with a B.A. degree, one year earlier than normal. In 1905, he received his M.A., also in German. However, Sapir’s linguistic interests proved to be much broader. In the next two years he took up projects studying the [[Chinookan|Wishram]] and [[Takelma language|Takelma]] languages. He earned his Ph.D. in [[anthropology]] in 1909, with a dissertation on the Takelma language of southwestern Oregon.
Sapir was born in Lauenburg, [[Germany]], now [[Lębork]] in [[Poland]], in 1884 to an orthodox Jewish family. In 1904 he graduated from [[Columbia University]] with a degree in Germanics, but his linguistic interests proved to be much broader. In the next two years he took up projects studying the [[Chinookan|Wishram]] and [[Takelma language|Takelma]] languages in the field. While at Columbia he met his mentor, anthropologist [[Franz Boas]], who was probably the person who provided the most initial impetus for Sapir's study of American languages. He arranged Sapir's employment in 1907-08 researching the nearly extinct [[Yahi|Yana]] language of northern California, to which he returned briefly in 1911 to work with [[Ishi]], the monolingual last surviving speaker of [[Yahi]] (southern Yana).
 
  
In the years 1910-1925 he built and directed the Anthropological Division in the Geological Survey of Canada, in Ottawa. When he was first hired, he and [[Marius Barbeau]] were the only two, and the first two, full-time anthropologists in Canada.  Among the many accomplishments of this very productive period are a substantial series of publications on Nootka and other languages, and his seminal book ''Language'' (1921), still important today and eminently readable. As he was leaving for a teaching position at the [[University of Chicago]], one of very few research universities then in the United States, he enabled [[Leonard Bloomfield]] to obtain support from Ottawa to do fieldwork on Cree, essential to his project of historical reconstruction in Algonkian. Bloomfield moved to Chicago in 1927 to teach Germanic languages. It appears (Darnell 268-272) that they were congenial but not close. From 1931 to his death Sapir was at [[Yale University]], where he became the head of the Department of Anthropology.  
+
While at Columbia, Sapir met his mentor, anthropologist [[Franz Boas]], who was probably the person who provided the initial impetus for Sapir's study of American languages. Boas invoked in Sapir an urgency to study and record endangered languages before they become lost forever. Boas arranged Sapir's employment in 1907-1908 researching the nearly extinct [[Yahi|Yana]] language of northern California.  
  
He was one of the first who explored the relations between language studies and anthropology.  His students include [[Li Fanggui|Fang-kuei Li]], [[Benjamin Whorf]], [[Mary Haas]], and [[Harry Hoijer]], but it was one not formally his student who he came to regard as his intellectual heir, a young [[Semitic Languages|Semiticist]] named [[Zellig Harris]] (who for a time dated his daughter).
+
Sapir’s first professional appointment was in 1907 at the [[University of California]], Berkeley, after which he moved in 1908 to the [[University of Pennsylvania]], where he taught until 1910. At the same time he conducted several field trips and studied the [[Ute]], Southern Paiute, and [[Hopi]] languages. In 1915, Sapir continued to work with Ishi, the last surviving monolingual speaker of Yahi, the language of the southern [[Yana]] tribe.  
  
Some suggestions of Sapir about the influence of language on the ways in which people think were adopted and developed by Whorf, initially while he was substitute teaching in the classroom during Sapir's illness. It was felt that stimulating and challenging ideas would attract students to this fledgling field. During the 1940s and later this became known as the [[Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis]]. Some support may be found in late work of Harris.  
+
In the years 1910-1925 he built and directed the Anthropological Division in the Geological Survey of Canada, in Ottawa. When he was first hired, he and Marius Barbeau were the first two, full-time anthropologists in [[Canada]]. Among the many accomplishments of this very productive period were a substantial series of publications on Nootka and other languages, and his seminal book ''Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech'' (1921/1955), still important today and eminently readable.
 +
 
 +
Sapir married Florence Delson in 1910, but the marriage suffered from the very beginning. Florence developed series of mental and physical ailments and eventually died in 1924. Sapir’s mother helped in raising his three children. During those depressing years Sapir dove into his inner self, starting to write [[poetry]], compose [[music]], and study [[psychology]]. However, he missed research and [[teaching]], which he could not do in Canada.
 +
 
 +
In 1925, he received a call from the [[University of Chicago]], one of very few research universities at that time in the United States. Before leaving for the teaching position in Chicago, he enabled [[Leonard Bloomfield]] to obtain support from Ottawa for fieldwork on the [[Cree]], essential to his project of historical reconstruction of [[Algonquian languages]].
 +
 
 +
In Chicago, Sapir could satisfy his intellectual hunger. His collaboration with interactional psychiatrist [[Harry Stack Sullivan]] and political scientist [[Harold D. Lasswell]] was particularly notable. Sapir’s sphere of interest widened from solely linguistics to include cultural studies, psychology, politics, and methodology. He remarried in 1927, to Jean McClenaghan, who was a student at the university at the time. They had two children.
 +
 
 +
From 1931 to his death, Sapir spent his time at [[Yale University]], where he became head of the department of anthropology. There, he opened the first Yale school of linguistics. However, he also encountered numerous problems in his work. The economic effects of the [[Great Depression]] which limited funding, and the [[anti-Semitism|anti-Semitic]] currents at Yale, were among the most obvious ones. Sapir’s health suffered greatly.  
  
 
Sapir died of heart problems in 1939, at age 55.
 
Sapir died of heart problems in 1939, at age 55.
  
His special focus among American languages was in the [[Athabaskan]] languages, a family he was especially fascinated by: "Dene is probably the son-of-a-bitchiest language in America to actually ''know''...most fascinating of all languages ever invented" (Krauss 1986:157). Among the languages and cultures studied by Sapir are [[Chinookan|Wishram Chinook]], [[Navajo language|Navajo]], [[Nootka]], [[Paiute]], [[Takelma language|Takelma]], and [[Yahi|Yana]].  Although noted for his work on American linguistics, he was also prolific in linguistics in general, as depicted by his book ''Language'', which provides everything from a grammar-typological classification of languages (with examples ranging from [[Chinese language|Chinese]] to [[Nuu-chah-nulth language|Nootka]]) to speculation on the phenomenon on language drift and the arbitrariness of associations between language, race, and culture. He was also at least a minor participant in the [[international auxiliary language]] movement; in his paper [http://www.geocities.com/Athens/Forum/5037/sapir.html ''The Function of an International Auxiliary Language''], Sapir argued for the benefits of a regular grammar and advocated a critical focus on the fundamentals of language unbiased by the idiosyncrasies of national languages in the choice of an international auxiliary language.
+
==Work==
 +
 
 +
Sapir’s special focus among [[Native American]] languages was on the [[Athabaskan]] languages, a family he was especially fascinated by: "Dene is probably the …most fascinating of all languages ever invented" (Krauss 1986:157). Other languages and [[culture]]s studied by Sapir include [[Wishram Chinook]], [[Navajo]], [[Nootka]], [[Paiute]], [[Takelma]], and [[Yana]].   
 +
 
 +
Although noted for his work on American linguistics, Sapir was also a prolific writer in [[linguistics]] in general, as depicted by his book ''Language,'' which provides everything from a grammar-typological classification of languages (with examples ranging from [[Chinese language|Chinese]] to Nootka) to speculation on the phenomenon on "language drift" and the arbitrariness of associations between language, race, and culture. He was also at least a minor participant in the international auxiliary language movement. Sapir argued for the benefits of a regular [[grammar]] and advocated a critical focus on the fundamentals of language unbiased by the idiosyncrasies of national languages in the choice of an international auxiliary language.
 +
 
 +
Besides pure linguistics, Sapir was also interested in cultural behaviorism and the development of [[personality]]. He searched for the connection between personality, verbal expression, and socially determined behavior. He saw language as a verbal [[symbol]] in human relations. Sapir believed that language shapes human [[perception]] and directs human behavior. From his view, understanding a culture was impossible without understanding the historical development of that culture’s language.
 +
 
 +
Sapir argued that language is not static, but that it constantly changes. Sapir called that change the "Language Drift." Some parts of language change quickly while some are much slower. As reality changes, so does the language. Conversely, due to the change in language, reality changes as well. We think, hear, see, and behave through our language. Language serves as a certain filter through which we experience and interpret reality. Every culture has its own language, or set of filters through which it predisposes its members to certain kinds of experience and [[thinking]]. Without language, it is difficult to imagine human life at all.
 +
 
 +
Some of Sapir's ideas about the influence of language on the ways in which people think were adopted and developed by [[Benjamin Whorf]], initially while he was substitute teaching in the classroom during Sapir's illness. Whorf's development of these ideas later became known as the "Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis," and laid the foundation for [[ethnolinguistics]] to develop as an academic field.
 +
 
 +
==Legacy==
  
==Books==
+
Edward Sapir was one of the first who explored the relationship between [[language]] and [[anthropology]]. His students include Li Fanggui, [[Benjamin Whorf]], Mary Haas, and Harry Hoijer. However, it was not one of his formal students whom he came to regard as his intellectual heir, but rather a young scholar of Semitic languages named [[Zellig S. Harris]], (who for a time dated his daughter).
*(ed.) ''Wishram Texts'' (1909)
 
*[http://www.gutenberg.org/etext/12629 ''Language: An introduction to the study of speech''] (1921) ISBN 0-15-648233-9 ([http://www.bartleby.com/186/ another link])
 
*with: M. Swadesh. ''Nootka texts: Tales and Ethnological Narratives with Grammatical Notes and Lexical Materials''. William Dwight Whitney Linguistic Series. Philadelphia: Linguistic Society of America 1939
 
*David G. Mandelbaum (ed.) ''Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture, and Personality''. Berkeley: University of California Press 1949. ISBN 0-520-01115-5.
 
  
==Essays and articles==
+
Sapir was an extremely well-liked scientist. His way of expression was simple and easy to understand, and he had a rather modest personality. Above all, he wanted to pass on his love of [[linguistics]] and anthropology to others. He abstained from seeking personal glory. His students liked him and, after his death, prepared numerous collections of their own essays, dedicating them to him.
*[http://www.geocities.com/Athens/Forum/5037/sapir.html ''The Function of an International Auxiliary Language''] (First published in ''Romanic Review'', July 1925. Also published in H. N. Shenton, E. Sapir and [[Otto Jespersen|O. Jespersen]], ''International Communication: A Symposium on the Language Problem'', London 1931, pp. 65-94.)
 
*''The problem of noun incorporation in American languages''. Am. Anthropol. 13:250-82. (1911)
 
*[http://spartan.ac.brocku.ca/~lward/Sapir/Sapir_1916.html ''Time Perspective in Aboriginal American Culture: A Study in Method'']. Canada Department of Mines, Geological Survey, Memoir 90. Anthropological Series, No. 13. (1916)
 
*"Sound patterns in language", in: ''Language'' 1 (1925), pp. 37-51 [= ''Selected Writings'', pp. 33-45]
 
*"The grammarian and his language", in: ''American Mercury'' 1 (1924), pp. 149-155 [= ''Selected Writings'', pp. 150-159]
 
*"La réalité psychologique des phonèmes" [The psychological reality of phonemes], in: ''Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique'' 30 (1933), pp. 247-265 [= ''Selected Writings'', pp. 46-60]
 
*"Grading, a study in Semantics", in: ''Philosophy of Science'' 11/2 (Apr. 1944), pp. 93-116 [= ''Selected Writings'', pp. 122-149]
 
  
==Bibliography==
+
==Publications==
*''Edward Sapir: Appraisals of His Life and Work''. Ed. Konrad Koerner. Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 1984. A collection of obituaries, bibliographic sketches, and critiques (mostly positive) of Sapir's work by his colleagues, students, and others affected by his work.
+
 
*''The Collected Works of Edward Sapir''. Ed. William Bright. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 1990. Volume IV presents more of his anthropological work, and makes reference to his work with Fang-Kuei Li. Volumes V and VI contain his work on American Indian languages.
+
*Sapir, Edward. 1974 (original 1909). ''Wishram Texts''. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404581528
*''Edward Sapir: Linguist, Anthropologist, Humanist.'' Regna Darnell. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press, 1990Darnell's book is the best source on Sapir's life and his work as an anthropologist.
+
*Sapir, Edward. 1911. "The problem of noun incorporation in American languages" in ''American Anthropology''. 13, 250-82.
*''Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis'' (Studies in the Social and Cultural Foundations of Language, No. 12). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1992. The most comprehensive overview of research into linguistic relativity.
+
*Sapir, Edward. 1916. ''Time Perspective in Aboriginal American Culture: A Study in Method''. Canada Department of Mines, Geological Survey, Memoir 90. Anthropological Series, 13.
*''New perspectives in language, culture, and personality: Proceedings of the Edward Sapir Centenary Conference (Ottawa, 1-3 October 1984)''. Ed. William Cowan, Michael K. Foster, Konrad Koerner. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 1986).
+
*Sapir, Edward. 1955 (original 1921). ''Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech''. Harvest Books. ISBN 015-648-2339
*Edward Sapir and Athabaskan linguistics, with preliminary annotated bibliography of Sapir's work on Athabaskan and Na-Dene. Michael E. Krauss. In W. Cowan, M. K. Foster, & K. Koerner 1986).
+
*Sapir, Edward. 1924. "The grammarian and his language" in ''American Mercury''. 1, 149-155.
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1925. "Sound patterns in language" in ''Language''. 1, 37-51.
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1925. [http://www.geocities.com/Athens/Forum/5037/sapir.html "The Function of an International Auxiliary Language"] ''Romanic Review''.
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1933. "La réalité psychologique des phonèmes" (The psychological reality of phonemes) in ''Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique''. 30, 247-265.
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1974 (original 1939). ''Nootka texts: Tales and Ethnological Narratives with Grammatical Notes and Lexical Materials''. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404118933
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1944. "Grading, a study in Semantics in ''Philosophy of Science''. 11(2), 93-116.
 +
*Sapir, Edward., Darnell, Regna., Irvine, Judith T., & Richard Handler. 1999. ''The Collected Works of Edward Sapir: Culture''. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3110126397
 +
 
 +
==References==
 +
 
 +
*Cowan, William & Michael K. Foster. 1986. ''New perspectives in language, culture, and personality''. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
 +
*Darnell, Regna. 1989. ''Edward Sapir: Linguist, Anthropologist, Humanist''. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. ISBN 0520066782
 +
*Koerner, Konrad & E.F.K. Koerner. 1985. ''Edward Sapir: Appraisals of His Life and Work''Philadelphia: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245185
 +
*Krauss, Michael E. 1986. "Edward Sapir and Athabaskan linguistics, with preliminary annotated bibliography of Sapir's work on Athabaskan and Na-Dene" in ''New perspectives in language, culture, and personality''. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
 +
*Lucy, John Arthur. 1992. ''Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis'' (Studies in the Social and Cultural Foundations of Language, No. 12). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.  ISBN 0521387973
 +
*Mandelbaum, David G. 1986. ''Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture, and Personality''. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-01115-5.
  
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
 +
All links retrieved February 12, 2024.
  
*[http://www.nap.edu/readingroom/books/biomems/esapir.html National Academy of Sciences biography]
+
*[http://www.bartleby.com/186/ ''Language: An introduction to the study of speech'']--Full text.  
*http://www.bartleby.com/people/Sapir-Ed.html
 
*http://spartan.ac.brocku.ca/~lward/Sapir/Documents.html
 
*http://www.yale.edu/linguist/Sapir.html
 
*http://www.mnsu.edu/emuseum/information/biography/pqrst/sapir_edward.html
 
* {{gutenberg author| id=Edward+Sapir | name=Edward Sapir}}
 
 
 
  
  
 
{{Credit1|Edward_Sapir|73661761|}}
 
{{Credit1|Edward_Sapir|73661761|}}

Latest revision as of 23:51, 12 February 2024


Edward Sapir (January 26, 1884 – February 4, 1939) was an American anthropologist-linguist, a leader in American structural linguistics. He is arguably the most influential figure in American linguistics. His anthropological approach, influenced by Franz Boas, began with his research on Native Americans in an attempt to document their languages before they disappeared. This led to his interest in the inter-connections among language, thinking, and culture. Work with Benjamin Whorf in this area led to what became known as the "Sapir-Whorf hypothesis" and the development of the field of ethnolinguistics. Sapir's belief that language shapes perception and influences behavior led him to research the nature of an international auxiliary language. In order to function as a harmonious global society, effective communication must be achieved, and Sapir's work was instrumental in developing the understanding of how people may communicate without misunderstanding.

Life

Edward Sapir was born on January 26, 1884 in Lauenburg, Prussia, (now Lębork in Poland) to an orthodox Jewish family of Lithuanian origins. His parents, Jacob David and Eva Seagal Sapir spoke Yiddish amongst themselves, so Edward learned both German and the language of his parents. When he was six years old his family immigrated to the United States, and eventually settled on the Lower East Side of New York City. After the death of his younger brother Max, and numerous problems, Sapir’s parents divorced in 1910.

Sapir entered Columbia University in 1901, on a Pulitzer scholarship he had won three years earlier. He was an extremely bright young man, and many professors saw great potential in him. At Columbia he studied German philology and Indo-European linguistics. In 1904, he graduated with a B.A. degree, one year earlier than normal. In 1905, he received his M.A., also in German. However, Sapir’s linguistic interests proved to be much broader. In the next two years he took up projects studying the Wishram and Takelma languages. He earned his Ph.D. in anthropology in 1909, with a dissertation on the Takelma language of southwestern Oregon.

While at Columbia, Sapir met his mentor, anthropologist Franz Boas, who was probably the person who provided the initial impetus for Sapir's study of American languages. Boas invoked in Sapir an urgency to study and record endangered languages before they become lost forever. Boas arranged Sapir's employment in 1907-1908 researching the nearly extinct Yana language of northern California.

Sapir’s first professional appointment was in 1907 at the University of California, Berkeley, after which he moved in 1908 to the University of Pennsylvania, where he taught until 1910. At the same time he conducted several field trips and studied the Ute, Southern Paiute, and Hopi languages. In 1915, Sapir continued to work with Ishi, the last surviving monolingual speaker of Yahi, the language of the southern Yana tribe.

In the years 1910-1925 he built and directed the Anthropological Division in the Geological Survey of Canada, in Ottawa. When he was first hired, he and Marius Barbeau were the first two, full-time anthropologists in Canada. Among the many accomplishments of this very productive period were a substantial series of publications on Nootka and other languages, and his seminal book Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech (1921/1955), still important today and eminently readable.

Sapir married Florence Delson in 1910, but the marriage suffered from the very beginning. Florence developed series of mental and physical ailments and eventually died in 1924. Sapir’s mother helped in raising his three children. During those depressing years Sapir dove into his inner self, starting to write poetry, compose music, and study psychology. However, he missed research and teaching, which he could not do in Canada.

In 1925, he received a call from the University of Chicago, one of very few research universities at that time in the United States. Before leaving for the teaching position in Chicago, he enabled Leonard Bloomfield to obtain support from Ottawa for fieldwork on the Cree, essential to his project of historical reconstruction of Algonquian languages.

In Chicago, Sapir could satisfy his intellectual hunger. His collaboration with interactional psychiatrist Harry Stack Sullivan and political scientist Harold D. Lasswell was particularly notable. Sapir’s sphere of interest widened from solely linguistics to include cultural studies, psychology, politics, and methodology. He remarried in 1927, to Jean McClenaghan, who was a student at the university at the time. They had two children.

From 1931 to his death, Sapir spent his time at Yale University, where he became head of the department of anthropology. There, he opened the first Yale school of linguistics. However, he also encountered numerous problems in his work. The economic effects of the Great Depression which limited funding, and the anti-Semitic currents at Yale, were among the most obvious ones. Sapir’s health suffered greatly.

Sapir died of heart problems in 1939, at age 55.

Work

Sapir’s special focus among Native American languages was on the Athabaskan languages, a family he was especially fascinated by: "Dene is probably the …most fascinating of all languages ever invented" (Krauss 1986:157). Other languages and cultures studied by Sapir include Wishram Chinook, Navajo, Nootka, Paiute, Takelma, and Yana.

Although noted for his work on American linguistics, Sapir was also a prolific writer in linguistics in general, as depicted by his book Language, which provides everything from a grammar-typological classification of languages (with examples ranging from Chinese to Nootka) to speculation on the phenomenon on "language drift" and the arbitrariness of associations between language, race, and culture. He was also at least a minor participant in the international auxiliary language movement. Sapir argued for the benefits of a regular grammar and advocated a critical focus on the fundamentals of language unbiased by the idiosyncrasies of national languages in the choice of an international auxiliary language.

Besides pure linguistics, Sapir was also interested in cultural behaviorism and the development of personality. He searched for the connection between personality, verbal expression, and socially determined behavior. He saw language as a verbal symbol in human relations. Sapir believed that language shapes human perception and directs human behavior. From his view, understanding a culture was impossible without understanding the historical development of that culture’s language.

Sapir argued that language is not static, but that it constantly changes. Sapir called that change the "Language Drift." Some parts of language change quickly while some are much slower. As reality changes, so does the language. Conversely, due to the change in language, reality changes as well. We think, hear, see, and behave through our language. Language serves as a certain filter through which we experience and interpret reality. Every culture has its own language, or set of filters through which it predisposes its members to certain kinds of experience and thinking. Without language, it is difficult to imagine human life at all.

Some of Sapir's ideas about the influence of language on the ways in which people think were adopted and developed by Benjamin Whorf, initially while he was substitute teaching in the classroom during Sapir's illness. Whorf's development of these ideas later became known as the "Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis," and laid the foundation for ethnolinguistics to develop as an academic field.

Legacy

Edward Sapir was one of the first who explored the relationship between language and anthropology. His students include Li Fanggui, Benjamin Whorf, Mary Haas, and Harry Hoijer. However, it was not one of his formal students whom he came to regard as his intellectual heir, but rather a young scholar of Semitic languages named Zellig S. Harris, (who for a time dated his daughter).

Sapir was an extremely well-liked scientist. His way of expression was simple and easy to understand, and he had a rather modest personality. Above all, he wanted to pass on his love of linguistics and anthropology to others. He abstained from seeking personal glory. His students liked him and, after his death, prepared numerous collections of their own essays, dedicating them to him.

Publications

  • Sapir, Edward. 1974 (original 1909). Wishram Texts. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404581528
  • Sapir, Edward. 1911. "The problem of noun incorporation in American languages" in American Anthropology. 13, 250-82.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1916. Time Perspective in Aboriginal American Culture: A Study in Method. Canada Department of Mines, Geological Survey, Memoir 90. Anthropological Series, 13.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1955 (original 1921). Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech. Harvest Books. ISBN 015-648-2339
  • Sapir, Edward. 1924. "The grammarian and his language" in American Mercury. 1, 149-155.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1925. "Sound patterns in language" in Language. 1, 37-51.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1925. "The Function of an International Auxiliary Language" Romanic Review.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1933. "La réalité psychologique des phonèmes" (The psychological reality of phonemes) in Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique. 30, 247-265.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1974 (original 1939). Nootka texts: Tales and Ethnological Narratives with Grammatical Notes and Lexical Materials. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404118933
  • Sapir, Edward. 1944. "Grading, a study in Semantics in Philosophy of Science. 11(2), 93-116.
  • Sapir, Edward., Darnell, Regna., Irvine, Judith T., & Richard Handler. 1999. The Collected Works of Edward Sapir: Culture. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3110126397

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Cowan, William & Michael K. Foster. 1986. New perspectives in language, culture, and personality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
  • Darnell, Regna. 1989. Edward Sapir: Linguist, Anthropologist, Humanist. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. ISBN 0520066782
  • Koerner, Konrad & E.F.K. Koerner. 1985. Edward Sapir: Appraisals of His Life and Work. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245185
  • Krauss, Michael E. 1986. "Edward Sapir and Athabaskan linguistics, with preliminary annotated bibliography of Sapir's work on Athabaskan and Na-Dene" in New perspectives in language, culture, and personality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
  • Lucy, John Arthur. 1992. Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis (Studies in the Social and Cultural Foundations of Language, No. 12). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0521387973
  • Mandelbaum, David G. 1986. Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture, and Personality. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-01115-5.

External links

All links retrieved February 12, 2024.


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.