Difference between revisions of "Unconscious mind" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
m
 
(37 intermediate revisions by 8 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 +
{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}{{Approved}}{{Paid}}{{copyedited}}
 +
{{psychoanalysis}}
  
 +
The '''Unconscious''' is a rich concept with a multi-faceted history. For [[Sigmund Freud|Freud]] it began as that part of the [[mind]] that contained our repressed [[anxiety|anxieties]], and later it developed into the site of repression for the [[Oedipus complex]], which is the illicit desire to sleep with one's mother and kill one's father. Still later, when Freud developed his structural model, it became the site not only of the [[Id]], but also the [[Superego]], which is the civilizing "[[instinct]]" that represented the legacy of the [[parent]]al voice, making both inaccessible to the functioning [[Ego]].
  
''For the physiological state of "being unconscious", as when knocked-out or asleep, see [[unconsciousness]].''
+
For [[Jacques Lacan|Lacan]] the unconscious was "structured like a language," and in reality it was [[language]], that is, that element which is already given and is not really available to subjectivity. For [[Carl Jung|Jung]] the unconscious contains both personal material that has been repressed or simply forgotten, but more importantly it contains the [[collective unconscious]], an accumulation of inherited experiences of all humankind that guides and advises our conscious mind. For [[cognitive psychology|cognitive psychologists]] it consists of processes and information that operate, without need for our conscious intervention, to enable us to make sense of the world.  
  
In [[psychoanalytic theory]], the '''unconscious''' refers to that part of mental functioning of which the [[subject (philosophy)|subject]] makes himself unaware. The psychoanalytic unconscious is similar to but not precisely the same as the popular notion of the [[subconscious]].
+
Many others reject the whole notion of an unconscious mind, regarding it as merely a social construction, denying the need to invoke mental processes that are not accessible, and arguing against the validity of such non-falsifiable theories. Still, various observers throughout history have argued that there are influences on [[consciousness]] from other parts of the [[mind]], invoking notions such as [[intuition]]. Other terms that relate to semi-conscious states or processes include: [[awake]]ning, [[implicit memory]], [[subliminal message]]s, [[trance]], and [[hypnosis]]. While [[sleep]], [[sleep walking]], [[delirium]], and [[coma]] may signal the presence of unconscious processes they may be different from an unconscious mind.
 +
{{toc}}
 +
Those who acknowledge the spiritual nature of human beings note that [[spiritual sense]]s permit people to communicate with the [[spiritual world]], providing access to information and processes that can be understood as a deeper level of each mind. However, for many people their spiritual senses are so dulled that they are generally unaware of them and their "spiritual mind," thus it has been relegated as the role of the unconscious.
  
For psychoanalysis, the unconscious does not include all of what is simply not conscious - it does not include e.g. motor skills - but rather, only what is actively [[psychological repression|repressed]] from conscious thought.
+
==Historical overview==
 +
===Ancient roots===
 +
The idea of an unconscious mind originated in antiquity <ref>More modern history is detailed in Henri F. Ellenberger's ''Discovery of the Unconscious'' (Basic Books 1970)</ref> and has been explored across [[culture]]s. It was recorded between 2500 and 600 B.C.E..E. in the Hindu texts known as the [[Vedas]], found today in [[Ayurveda|Ayurvedic]] medicine.<ref>Alexander, C. N. Growth of Higher Stages of Consciousness: Maharishi's Vedic Psychology of Human Development. C. N. Alexander and E.J. Langer (eds.). Higher Stages of Human Development. Perspectives on Human Growth. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1990.</ref> <ref>Meyer-Dinkgräfe, D. Consciousness and the Actor. A Reassessment of Western and Indian Approaches to the Actor's Emotional Involvement from the Perspective of Vedic Psychology. Frankfurt/Main: Peter Lang, 1996. </ref> <ref>Haney, W. S. II. Unity in Vedic aesthetics: the self-interacting dynamics of the knower, the known, and the process of knowing. Analecta Husserliana, 233, 295-319, 1991.</ref> <ref>Geraldine Coster 'Yoga and Western Psychology: A comparison' 1934.</ref> In the Vedic worldview, consciousness is the basis of physiology <ref>Wallace, R. K.; Fagan, J. B.; and Pasco, D. S. Vedic physiology. Modern Science and Vedic Science 2 (1): 3-59, 1988.</ref> <ref>King, Michael S. Natural Law and the Bhagavad-Gita: The Vedic Concept of Natural Law Ratio Juris 16 (3), 399–415, 2003.</ref> and pure consciousness is "an abstract, silent, completely unified field of consciousness" <ref>Alexander, Charles N, Robert W. Cranson, Robert W. Boyer, David W. Orme-Johnson. "Transcendental Consciousness: A Fourth State of consciousness beyond Sleep, Dream, and Waking."  Sleep and Dream. Sourcebook. Ed. Jayne Gackenbach. New York, London: Garland Publishing Inc., 282-315, 1986.</ref> within "an architecture of increasingly abstract, functionally integrated faculties or levels of mind." <ref>Alexander Charles N. et al. "Growth of Higher Stages of Consciousness: Maharishi's Vedic Psychology of Human Development." Higher Stages of Human Development. Perspectives on Human Growth. Eds. Charles N. Alexander and Ellen J. Langer. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 286-341, 1990.</ref>
  
As defined by [[Sigmund Freud]], the [[psyche]] is composed of different levels of consciousness, often
+
===Literary roots===
 +
[[William Shakespeare]] explored the role of the unconscious <ref>''The Design Within: Psychoanalytic Approaches to Shakespeare'' Edited by M. D. Faber. New York, NY: Science House, 1970. An anthology of 33 papers on Shakespearean plays by psychoanalysts and literary critics whose work has been influenced by psychoanalysis.</ref> in many of his plays, without naming it as such. <ref>Meyer-Dinkgräfe, Daniel "Hamlet’s Procrastination: A Parallel to the Bhagavad-Gita," in ''Hamlet East West'', edited by Marta Gibinska and Jerzy Limon. Gdansk: Theatrum Gedanese Foundation, 187-195, 1998.</ref> <ref>Meyer-Dinkgräfe, Daniel 'Consciousness and the Actor: A Reassessment of Western and Indian Approaches to the Actor's Emotional Involvement from the Perspec tive of Vedic Psychology.' Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang, (Series 30: Theatre, Film and Television, Vol. 67) 1996. </ref> <ref>Yarrow, Ralph 'Identity and Consciousness East and West: the case of Russell Hoban'. Journal of Literature & Aesthetics, Vol. 5, No. 2, 19-26, 1997.</ref> In the nineteenth century [[Gothic fiction]] also treated the unconscious mind in such works as [[Robert Louis Stevenson]]'s ''Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde''.
  
For Freud, the unconscious was a depository for socially unacceptable ideas, wishes or desires, traumatic memories, and painful emotions put out of mind by the mechanism of [[psychological repression]]. However, the contents did not necessarily have to be solely negative. In the psychoanalytic view, the unconscious is a force that can only be recognized by its effects - it expresses itself in the [[symptom]].
+
===The unconscious in philosophy===
 +
Western philosophers, such as [[Baruch Spinoza|Spinoza]], [[Gottfried Leibniz|Leibniz]], [[Arthur Schopenhauer|Schopenhauer]], and [[Friedrich Nietzsche|Nietzsche]], developed a western view of mind which foreshadowed those of [[Sigmund Freud|Freud]]'s thought. Schopenhauer was also influenced by his reading of the Vedas and the [[Jewish]] [[mysticism]] of the [[Kabbalah]]. Freud drew on his own Jewish roots to develop an interpersonal examination of the unconscious mind <ref>Drob, S. Freud and the Chasidim: Redeeming The Jewish Soul of Psychoanalysis. Jewish Review 3:1, 1989.</ref> <ref>Drob, Sanford L. "This is Gold": Freud, Psychotherapy and the Lurianic Kabbalah (HTML), (1998-2006). Retrieved April 4, 2007.</ref> <ref>Drob, Sanford L. "Jung and the Kabbalah" (HTML). History of Psychology. May, 1999 Vol 2(2) 102-118. Retrieved April 4, 2007.</ref> as well as his own therapeutic roots in [[hypnosis]] into an apparently new therapeutic intervention and its associated rationale, known as [[psychoanalysis]].
  
At the present stage, there are still fundamental disagreements within psychology about the nature of the unconscious mind (if indeed it is considered to exist at all), whereas outside formal psychology a whole world of pop-psychological speculation has grown up in which the unconscious mind is held to have any number of properties and abilities, from animalistic and innocent, child-like aspects to [[savant]]-like, all-perceiving, [[mysticism|mystical]] and [[occult]]ic properties.
+
Articulating the idea of something not conscious or actively denied to awareness with the symbolic [[culture|constructs]] of [[language]] has been a process of human thought and interpersonal [[influence]] for over a thousand years. Freud and his followers popularized unconscious motivation in a culture of the [[Individualism|individual]] and within a philosophical tradition that emphasized the [[Subject]], which posited a self viewed as both separate and sufficient.
  
==Pre-Freudian history of the idea==
+
The resultant status of the unconscious mind may be viewed as a [[social construction]]&ndash;that the unconscious exists because people agree to behave as if it exists. <ref>Peter L. Berger and Thomas Luckmann: The Social Construction of Reality. New York: Doubleday, 1966.</ref> [[Symbolic interactionism]] discusses this further and argues that people's selves (conscious and unconscious) are although purposeful and creative are nevertheless social products. <ref>Blumer, Herbert. "Society as Symbolic Interaction," in Arnold M. Rose: Human Behavior and Social Process: An Interactionist Approach. Houghton-Mifflin, 1999.</ref>
Although the idea of the unconscious is generally attributed to Freud, the notion of the unconscious originated in antiquity. The Romantic philosophers and writers helped to popularize the notion from the beginning of the nineteenth century. [[Gottfried Leibniz|Leibniz]], [[Arthur Schopenhauer|Schopenhauer]], and later [[Friedrich Nietzsche|Nietzsche] all developed ideas that foreshadowed the modern conception of the unconscious, as did writers such as [[Mary Shelley]] and [[Robert Louis Stevenson]]. Its more modern history is detailed in Henri F. Ellenberger's ''Discovery of the Unconscious'' (Basic Books, 1970).  
 
  
Medical researchers also made important contributions. With the rise of industrialization, industrial accidents helped provide a curious background to Freud's "discovery" of the unconscious mind. Railway accidents in particular were significant, as doctors would often discover that some victims of these accidents with no organic trauma would nonetheless experience symptoms similar to those with physical wounds. This opened to door to lots of investigations and speculation about the nature of the unconscious mind. One such researcher was [[Jean-Marie Charcot]], one of Freud's teachers and mentors. Charcot became interested in hysteria, as he noticed that his hysterical patients had symptoms that mimicked those of his epileptic patients. Based on that finding, he experimented with hypnosis, discovering that he could introduce symptoms and remove them through the use of the hypnotic trance. Freud studied with Charcot, becoming interested in the problem of hysteria.
+
== Unconscious process and unconscious mind ==
  
Freud's view of the unconscious was influenced by his time spent at Charcot's clinic (1885-6), the Salpetriere, and his use of hypnosis on his patients to induce and remove hysterical symptoms. Freud confesses that "I received the profoundest impression of the possibility that there could be powerful mental processes which nevertheless remained hidden from the consciousness of men." (Autobiography, S.E. 20:17)
+
[[Neuroscience]], while an unlikely place to find support for a [[proposition]] as adaptable as the unconscious mind, <ref> Miller, Laurence In search of the unconscious; evidence for some cornerstones of Freudian theory is coming from an unlikely source - basic neuroscience. Psychology Today, December 1, 1986.</ref> has nonetheless produced some interesting results. "Researchers at Columbia University Medical Center have found that fleeting images of fearful faces&mdash;images that appear and disappear so quickly that they escape conscious awareness&mdash;produce unconscious anxiety that can be detected in the brain with the latest neuroimaging machines."<ref>[http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/medicalnews.php?newsid=18022&nfid=rssfeeds Fleeting Images of Fearful Faces Reveal Neurocircuitry of Unconscious Anxiety] Retrieved November 19, 2007.</ref> The [[consciousness|conscious mind]] is hundreds of milliseconds behind those unconscious processes.
  
 +
While these results represent [[Scientific method|research]] into the unconscious processes of the mind, a distinction has to be drawn between unconscious processes and the unconscious mind. They are not identical. The results of neuroscience cannot demonstrate the existence of the unconscious mind. The unconscious mind and its expected psychoanalytic contents <ref>Crews, F.C. (Ed.) Unauthorized Freud: Doubters confront a legend. New York: Viking, 1998.</ref> <ref>Kihlstrom, J.F. Psychodynamics and social cognition: Notes on the fusion of psychoanalysis and psychology. ''Journal of Personality'', 62, 681-696, 1994.</ref> <ref>Kihlstrom, J.F. The psychological unconscious. In L.R. Pervin & O. John (Eds.), ''Handbook of personality'', 2nd ed. New York: Guilford, 424-442, 1999.</ref> <ref>Macmillan, M.B. Freud evaluated: The completed arc. Cambridge, Ma.: MIT Press, 1996. </ref> <ref>Roth, M. ''Freud: Conflict and culture''. New York, NY: Knopf, 1998.</ref> <ref>Westen, D. The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed psychological science. ''Psychological Bulletin'', 124, 333-371, 1998.</ref> are also different from [[unconsciousness]], [[coma]], and a [[minimally conscious state]]. [[Psychoanalysis|Psychoanalytic theory]] is, at best, a metanarrative on the way the mind functions, and not the result of scientific findings.<ref>Kihlstrom, J.F. Is Freud Still Alive? No, Not Really Retrieved from [http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~kihlstrm/freuddead.htm] April 17, 2007. Extract: No empirical evidence supports any specific proposition of psychoanalytic theory, such as the idea that development proceeds through oral, anal, phallic, and genital stages, or that little boys lust after their mothers and hate and fear their fathers. No empirical evidence indicates that psychoanalysis is more effective, or more efficient, than other forms of psychotherapy, such as systematic desensitization or assertiveness training. No empirical evidence indicates the mechanisms by which psychoanalysis achieves its effects, such as they are, are those specifically predicated on the theory, such as transference and catharsis.</ref>
  
 +
==The psychoanalytic unconscious==
 +
===Structure of the unconscious===
 +
The term "unconscious mind" is most closely associated with [[Sigmund Freud]] and his followers. It lies at the heart of [[psychoanalysis]].
  
 +
[[Consciousness]], in Freud's [[topographical]] view (which was his first of several psychological models of the [[mind]]) was a relatively thin [[perception|perceptual]] aspect of the mind, whereas the subconscious was that merely [[autonomic function]] of the [[brain]]. The unconscious was considered by Freud throughout the evolution of his [[psychoanalytic]] theory a [[sentient]] force of [[will]] influenced by human [[drive (psychoanalysis)|drive]] and yet operating well below the perceptual [[conscious mind]]. For Freud, the unconscious is the storehouse of instinctual desires, needs, and psychic actions. While past thoughts and memories may be deleted from immediate consciousness, they direct the thoughts and feelings of the individual from the realm of the unconscious. In this early view, the psychic struggle exists between the instinctual forces of the unconscious against the social demands of the conscious mind.
  
===The Freudian unconscious===
+
In this theory, the unconscious refers to that part of mental functioning of which [[subject (philosophy)|subjects]] make themselves unaware. <ref> Geraskov, Emil Asenov. The internal contradiction and the unconscious sources of activity. ''The Journal of Psychology'' November 1, 1994. Abstract: This article is an attempt to give new meaning to well-known experimental studies, analysis of which may allow us to discover unconscious behavior that has so far remained unnoticed by researchers. Those studies confirm many of the statements by Freud, but they also reveal new aspects of the unconscious psychic. The first global psychological concept of the internal contradiction as an unconscious factor influencing human behavior was developed by Sigmund Freud. In his opinion, this contradiction is expressed in the struggle between the biological instincts and the self. Retrieved from [http://www.highbeam.com/doc/1G1-16528826.html] April 17, 2007.</ref>
  
Freud does not have a singular theory of the unconscious. His views changed over time as his theories changed. In his early topographical theory, which dominated his view from roughly 1890 to 1920, the division of consciousness was tripartite. The three parts were designated as follows:
+
Freud proposed a vertical and [[hierarchical]] architecture of human consciousness: the [[conscious mind]], the [[preconscious]], and the unconscious [[mind]]&ndash;each lying beneath the other. He believed that significant psychic events take place "below the surface" in the unconscious mind.<ref>For example, [[dreams|dreaming]]: Freud called dream symbols the "royal road to the unconscious."</ref>, like hidden messages from the unconscious&ndash;a form of [[intrapersonal communication]] out of [[awareness]]. He [[Interpretation|interpreted]] these dream events as both symbolic and actual significance.
  
*the waking [[consciousness]]
+
In his later structural theory, as a response to the development of the ego theories of his former protégés like [[Carl Jung]] and [[Alfred Adler]], Freud divided the mind into the conscious mind or [[ego]] and two parts of the Unconscious: the [[id]] or [[instinct]]s and the [[superego]]. In this later construct, the unconscious part of the mind was expanded to include not only the instinctual desire of the id, but also the superego which represents the legacy of parental conditioning. In this model, the ego is mediator between id and superego.
*preconsciousness
 
*the unconscious proper
 
  
However, early psychoanalytic theory was essentially dualistic. The preconscious refers to those ideas, memories, etc. that are not currently conscious, but which are capable of becoming conscious by merely turning our conscious attention to them. The conflict that drives Freud's early psychoanalytic theory is the one between consciousness and the unconscious, which is defined as that which is not capable of becoming conscious. For example, we don't have memories from our first few years in life because memories require some conceptual ability and linguistic ability that babies lack. Yet, according to Freud, events that happen in early childhood have an enormous influence on the development of the personality. Even more important are those ideas and desires or impulses that must be repressed. The Freudian unconscious is unavailable to consciousness due to [[repression]]. It exists as a product of repression.  
+
===The meaning of the unconscious===
 +
In Freud's earlier model, the unconscious does not include all that is not conscious, but rather only what is actively repressed from conscious thought or what the person is averse to knowing consciously. That is, the part of the unconscious that is in conflict with conscious awareness. For Freud, the unconscious was a repository for socially unacceptable ideas, wishes or desires, traumatic memories, and painful emotions put out of mind by the mechanism of [[psychological repression]]. However, the contents did not necessarily have to be solely negative. In the psychoanalytic view, the unconscious is a force that can only be recognized by its effects&ndash;it expresses itself in the [[symptom]].
  
Freud's theory of the personality is based on a system of cathexis and anti-cathexis, the libido. The libido seeks an object which releases the energy. A buildup of energy creates tension, creating anxiety. But not every release of tension is socially acceptable to in civilization. Some desires have to be repressed because they are socially unacceptable.
+
In a sense, this view places the [[self]] in relationship to their unconscious as an adversary, warring with itself to keep what is unconscious hidden. The therapist is then a mediator trying to allow the unspoken or unspeakable to reveal itself using the tools of psychoanalysis. Messages arising from a conflict between conscious and unconscious are likely to be [[Cryptography|cryptic]], in the form of slips of the tongue or symptoms that require decoding. The psychoanalyst is presented as an [[expert]] in interpreting those messages. Unconscious thoughts are not directly accessible to ordinary [[introspection]], but are supposed to be capable of being "tapped" and "interpreted" by special methods and techniques such as random association, dream analysis, and [[Speech communication|verbal]] slips (commonly known as a [[Freudian slip]]), examined and conducted during [[psychoanalysis]].  
  
Repression exists in two forms, the primal repression and repression proper. The primal repression refers to the repression of the (male) child's Oedipal desire to possess the mother. This is the source of what Freud calls the incest taboo. According to Freud's theory of the [[Oedipus complex]] every (male) wants to possess his mother, but due to the demands of civilization expressed in the incest taboo, the Oedipal desire is repressed into the unconscious. In addition to this primal repression, which forms the basis of civilization, since society could not function were every male child to fulfill this desire, there are other socially unacceptable desires that must also be repressed. The function that allows this material to be repressed is the unconscious.
+
This model was problematized by the structural theory, which viewed the superego as another element of the unconscious. In this view, the ego is a staging ground for the battle between the unsocial, even anti-social, demands of the id and the superego, representing the parental, social conscience. On adopting this model, Freud began to eschew talk of a "psychoanalytic cure." The role of the analyst remained to make what was unconscious conscious, but Freud realized that the result of this knowledge would not be a cure.
  
Nonetheless, the unconscious erupts into the conscious world, but in marginal ways, such as in dreams, slips of the tongue and etc. Freud referred to [[dreams|dreaming]] as the "royal road to the unconscious".
+
==Post-Freudian unconscious==
 +
Freud's theory of the unconscious was substantially transformed by some of his followers, notable among them [[Carl Jung]] and [[Jacques Lacan]].
  
By 1895 Freud and Breuer, who were working on Studies in Hysteria together, came to the conclusion that the "strangulation of affect" was caused by three unconscious processes:
+
===Jung's collective unconscious===
 +
{{main|Collective unconscious|Carl Jung}}
 +
Carl Jung developed his unconscious concept in an entirely different direction than Freud. He divided the unconscious into two parts: the personal unconscious and the collective unconscious. The personal unconscious is a reservoir of material that was once conscious but has been forgotten or suppressed. This material is unique to the individual, a product of their [[personality]] and experience. There is a considerable two way traffic between the ego and the personal unconscious. For example, our [[attention]] can wander from this printed page to a [[memory]] of something we did yesterday.
  
*strong affect experienced during "hypnoid states." (Anna 0.)
+
The collective unconscious is the deepest level of the psyche containing the accumulation of inherited experiences. It is common to everyone. The collective unconscious has a better sense of the self ideal than the [[ego]] or conscious self has, and thus directs the self, via [[archetypes]], [[dream]]s, and [[intuition]], to self-actualization. Jung called the unconscious the great friend, guide, and adviser of the [[consciousness|conscious]].
*inadequate conscious discharge of affect
 
*ego defense mechanism against unwanted ideas (sexual)
 
  
 +
===Lacan's linguistic unconscious===
 +
{{main|Jacques Lacan}}
 +
Jacques Lacan's psychoanalytic theory, based on the rise of modern [[Structuralism]], contends that the unconscious is structured like a [[language]].
 +
 +
The unconscious, Lacan argued, was not a more primitive or archetypal part of the mind separate from the conscious, linguistic ego, but rather, a formation every bit as complex and linguistically sophisticated as consciousness itself.
 +
 +
Lacan argues that if the unconscious is structured like a language, then the [[self]] is denied any point of reference to which to be 'restored' following [[Psychological trauma|trauma]] or '[[identity crisis]]'. In this way, Lacan's thesis of the structurally dynamic unconscious is also a challenge to the [[ego psychology]] of [[Anna Freud]] and her American followers.
 +
 +
Lacan's theory is based on the [[structural linguistics]] of [[Ferdinand de Saussure]] and [[Roman Jakobson]], based on the function of the [[signifier]] and [[signified]] in [[signifying chain]]s. This has left Lacan's model of mental functioning open to severe critique, since in mainstream linguistics Saussurean models have largely been replaced.
 +
 +
The starting point for the linguistic theory of the unconscious was a re-reading of Freud's ''[[The Interpretation of Dreams]]''. There, Freud identifies two mechanisms at work in the formation of unconscious fantasies: condensation and displacement. Under Lacan's linguistic reading, condensation is identified with the linguistic trope of [[metonymy]], and displacement with [[metaphor]].
 +
 +
Lacan applied the ideas of de Saussure and Jakobson to psychoanalytic practice. However, while de Saussure described the linguistic sign as a relationship between a signified and an arbitrary signifier, Lacan inverted the relationship, putting in first place the signifier as determining the signified, making it closer to Freud's position that human beings know what they say only as a result of a chain of signifiers, a-posteriori. Lacan began this work with Freud's case study of Emma (1895), whose symptoms were disenchained in a two-phase temporal process. Lacan's approach brought Freud in greater proximity to the [[structuralism|structuralist]] and [[Post-structuralism|post-structuralist]] theories of modernity. For Lacan, modernity is the era when humans begin to grasp their essential dependence on language.
 +
 +
===Controversy===
 +
Today, there are still fundamental disagreements within [[psychology]] about the nature of the unconscious mind. Outside formal psychology, a whole world of pop-psychological speculation has grown up in which the unconscious mind is held to have any number of properties and abilities, from animalistic and innocent, child-like aspects to [[savant]]-like, all-perceiving, [[mysticism|mystical]] and [[occult]]ic properties.
 +
 +
The unconscious may simply stand as a metaphor that ought not to be taken literally. There is a great controversy over the concept of an unconscious in regard to its scientific or rational validity and whether the unconscious mind exists at all. Among philosophers, is [[Karl Popper]], one of Freud's most notable contemporary opponents. Popper argued that Freud's theory of the unconscious was not [[Falsifiability|falsifiable]], and therefore not [[science|scientific]]. He objected not so much to the idea that things happened in our minds that we are unconscious of; he objected to investigations of mind that were not falsifiable. If one could connect every imaginable experimental outcome with Freud's theory of the unconscious mind, then no [[experiment]] could refute the theory.
 +
 +
Unlike Popper, the [[epistemology|epistemologist]] [[Adolf Grunbaum]] has argued that psychoanalysis could be falsifiable, but its evidence has serious epistemological problems. David Holmes <ref>List of his publications at [http://www.geocities.com/psydic/DH_WEB/publicat.html] Retrieved April 18, 2007.</ref> examined sixty years of research about the Freudian concept of “repression,” and concluded that there is no positive evidence for this concept. Given the lack of evidence of many Freudian hypotheses, some scientific researchers proposed the existence of unconscious mechanisms that are very different from the Freudian ones. They speak of a “cognitive unconscious” (John Kihlstrom), <ref>Kihlstrom, J.F. The unconscious. In V.S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Human Brain, Vol. 4, 635-646. San Diego, CA: Academic, 2002.</ref> <ref>Kihlstrom, J.F., J.S. Beer, and S.B. Klein. Self and identity as memory. In M.R. Leary & J. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity. New York: Guilford Press. 68-90, 2002.</ref> an “adaptive unconscious” ([[Timothy Wilson]]), <ref>Wilson, T. D. Strangers to Ourselves Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious.</ref> or a “dumb unconscious” (Loftus & Klinger) <ref>Loftus, E. F., and M.R. Klinger. Is the Unconscious Smart or Dumb? American Psychologist, 47(6), 761-765, 1992.</ref> that executes automatic processes but lacks the complex mechanisms of repression and symbolic return of the repressed.
  
Freud's concept was a more subtle and complex psychological theory than many. Consciousness, in Freud's topographical view (which was his first of several psychological models of the mind) was a relatively thin perceptual aspect of the mind, whereas the subconscious (frequently misused and confused with the unconscious) was that merely autonomic function of the brain. The unconscious was indeed considered by Freud throughout the evolution of his psychoanalytic theory a sentient force of will influenced by human drives and yet operating well below the perceptual conscious mind. Hidden, like the man behind the curtain in the "Wizard of Oz," the unconscious directs the thoughts and feelings of everyone, according to Freud. This unconscious mind is the primitive instinctual hangover we all suffer from and which we must overcome in a healthy way in order to become fully and normally developed, i.e., not [[neurosis|neurotic]] or [[psychosis|psychotic]] but merely unhappy (See Frank Sulloway's ''Freud, Biologist of the Mind'', Basic Books, 1983).  
+
[[Ludwig Wittgenstein]] and [[Jacques Bouveresse]] argued that Freudian thought exhibits a systemic confusion between reasons and causes; the method of interpretation can give reasons for new meanings, but are useless to find causal relations (which require experimental research). Wittgenstein gave the following example (in his Conversations with Rush Rhees), "if we throw objects on a table, and we give free associations and interpretations about those objects, we’ll find a meaning for each object and its place, but we won’t find the causes."
  
In another of Freud's systematizations, the mind is divided into the conscious mind or [[Ego]] and two parts of the Unconscious: the [[Ego, Superego and Id|Id]] or [[instinct]]s and the [[Superego]]. Freud used the idea of the unconscious in order to explain certain kinds of neurotic behavior. (See [[psychoanalysis]].)
+
In the [[social science]]s, [[John B. Watson|John Watson]], who is considered the first American [[behaviorism|behaviorist]], criticized the idea of an "unconscious mind," along similar lines of reasoning, and focused on observable behaviors rather than on [[introspection]]. Other early psychologists, such as the [[experimental psychology|experimental psycholgist]] [[Wilhelm Wundt]], regarded psychology as the scientific study of immediate experience, and thus the study of human [[consciousness]], or the [[mind]], as long as mind is understood as the totality of conscious experience at a given moment. Wundt denied the role of unconscious processes, defining psychology as the study of conscious, and therefore observable, states.
  
Freud's theory of the unconscious was substantially transformed by some of his followers, among them [[Carl Jung]] and [[Jacques Lacan]].
+
Other critics of Freudian unconscious were [[Hans Eysenck]], Jacques Van Rillaer, Frank Cioffi, Marshal Edelson, and Edward Erwin. Some stress, however, that these critics did not grasp the real importance of Freud conceptions, and instead tried to criticize Freud on the basis of other fields.  
  
===Jung's [[collective unconscious]]===
+
In modern [[cognitive psychology]], many researchers have sought to strip the notion of the unconscious from its Freudian heritage, and alternative terms such as 'implicit' or 'automatic' have come into currency.  These traditions emphasize the degree to which cognitive processing happens outside the scope of cognitive awareness and how what we are unaware of can influence other cognitive processes as well as behavior. <ref>Greenwald, Anthony G., Sean C. Draine, and Richard L. Abrams. Three Cognitive Markers of Unconscious Semantic Activation Science. Vol. 273, 1699-1702. September 20, 1996.</ref> <ref>Gaillard, Raphaël. Antoine Del Cul, Lionel Naccache Fabien Vinckier, Laurent Cohen, and Stanislas Dehaene. Nonconscious semantic processing of emotional words modulates conscious access. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. Vol. 103, 7524-7529. May 9, 2006. Retrieved from [http://www.pnas.org/cgi/content/abstract/103/19/7524] April 17, 2007.</ref> <ref>Kiefer, Markus and Doreen Brendel. Attentional Modulation of Unconscious "Automatic" Processes: Evidence from Event-related Potentials in a Masked Priming Paradigm Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18:184-198 Retrieved from [http://jocn.mitpress.org/cgi/content/abstract/18/2/184] April 17, 2007.</ref> <ref>Naccache, L., R. Gaillard, C. Adam, D. Hasboun, S. Clemenceau, M. Baulac, S. Dehaene, and L. Cohen. A direct intracranial record of emotions evoked by subliminal words. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. Vol. 102, 7713-7717. May 24, 2005. Retrieved from [http://www.pnas.org/cgi/content/abstract/102/21/7713] April 17, 2007.</ref> <ref>Smith, E.R. and J. DeCoster. Dual-Process Models in Social and Cognitive Psychology: Conceptual Integration and Links to Underlying Memory Systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review 4, 108-131, 2000.</ref>  Active research traditions related to the unconscious include implicit memory (for example, [[priming]] or [[attitude]]) and non-conscious acquisition of knowledge (such as work by [[Pawel Lewicki]]).
  
[[Carl Jung]] developed the concept further. He divided the unconscious into two parts: the personal unconscious and the [[collective unconscious]]. The first of these corresponds to Freud's idea of the subconscious, though unlike his mentor, Jung believed that the personal unconscious contained a valuable counter-balance to the conscious mind, as well as childish urges. As for the collective unconscious, which consists of [[archetypes]], this is the common store of mental building blocks that makes up the psyche of all humans. Evidence for its existence is the universality of certain symbols that appear in the mythologies of nearly all peoples.
+
==Unconscious mind in contemporary cognitive psychology==
 +
===Research===
 +
While historically the psychoanalytic research tradition was the first to focus on the phenomenon of unconscious mental activity (and still the term "unconsciousness" or "the subconscious," for many, appears to be not only deeply rooted in, but almost synonymous with psychoanalytic tradition), there is an extensive body of research in contemporary [[cognitive psychology]] devoted to mental activity that is not mediated by conscious awareness.
  
===Lacan's linguistic unconscious===
+
Most of the cognitive research on unconscious processes has been done in the mainstream, academic tradition of the [[information processing]] paradigm. As opposed to the psychoanalytic tradition, driven by the relatively speculative (that is, empirically unverifiable), theoretical concepts such as [[Oedipus complex]] or [[Electra complex]], the cognitive tradition of research on unconscious processes is based on relatively few theoretical assumptions and based on empirical research. Cognitive research has demonstrated that outside of conscious awareness, individuals automatically register and acquire more information than they can experience through their conscious thoughts.
[[Jacques Lacan]]'s [[psychoanalytic theory]] contends that the unconscious is structured like a language.  
 
  
The unconscious, Lacan argued, was not a more primitive or archetypal part of the mind separate from the conscious, linguistic ego, but rather, a formation every bit as complex and linguistically sophisticated as consciousness itself.  (Compare [[collective unconscious]]).
+
===Unconscious processing of information about frequency===
  
If the unconscious is structured like a language, Lacan argues, then the self is denied any point of reference to which to be 'restored' following trauma or 'identity crisis'. In this way, Lacan's thesis of the structurally dynamic unconscious is also a challenge to the [[ego psychology]] that [[Freud]] himself opposed.
+
Hasher and Zacks demonstrated that outside of conscious awareness and without engaging conscious information processing resources individuals register information about the frequency of events.<ref>Hasher, L., and R.T. Zacks. Automatic processing of fundamental information: The case of frequency of occurrence. ''American Psychologist'', 39, 1372-1388, 1984.</ref>  Moreover, their research demonstrated that perceivers do that unintentionally, regardless of the instructions they receive, and regardless of the information processing goals they have. Interestingly, their ability to unconsciously and relatively accurately tally the frequency of events has appeared to have little or no relation to the individual's age, education, intelligence, or personality. Thus, this ability may represent one of the fundamental building blocks of human orientation in the environment and possibly the acquisition of [[procedural knowledge]] and experience.
  
Lacan's idea of how language is structured is largely taken from the [[structural linguistics]] of [[Ferdinand de Saussure]] and [[Roman Jakobson]], based on the function of the [[signifier]] and [[signified]] in [[signifying chain]]s.  This may leave Lacan's entire model of mental functioning open to severe critique, since in mainstream linguistics, Saussurean models have largely been replaced by those of e.g. [[Noam Chomsky]].
+
===Artificial grammars===
  
The starting point for the linguistic theory of the unconscious was a re-reading of Freud's ''[[The Interpretation of Dreams]]''.  There, Freud identifies two mechanisms at work in the formation of unconscious fantasies: condensation and displacement. Under Lacan's linguistic reading, condensation is identified with the linguistic trope of [[metonymy]], and displacement with [[metaphor]].
+
Another line of (non-psychoanalytic) early research on unconscious processes was initiated by Arthur Reber, using so-called "artificial grammar" methodology. That research revealed that individuals exposed to novel words created by complex set of artificial, synthetic "grammatical" rules (such as GKHAH, KHABT, and so forth), quickly develop some sort of a "feel" for that grammar and subsequent working knowledge of that grammar, as demonstrated by their ability to differentiate between new grammatically "correct" (consistent with the rules) and "incorrect" (inconsistent) words. Interestingly, that ability does not appear to be mediated, or even accompanied by the [[declarative knowledge]] of the rules—individuals' ability to articulate how they distinguish between the correct and incorrect words.
  
==Controversy==
+
===Unconscious acquisition of procedural knowledge===
  
Many modern philosophers and social scientists either dispute the concept of an unconscious, or argue that it is not something that can be scientifically investigated or discussed rationally. In the social sciences, this view was first brought forward by [[John B. Watson|John Watson]], considered to be the first American behaviourist. Among philosophers, [[Karl Popper]] was one of Freud's most notable contemporary opponents. Popper argued that Freud's theory of the unconscious was not [[Falsifiability|falsifiable]], and therefore not scientifical. However, critics of Popper have underlined that Popper's exclusion of psychoanalysis from the normal domain of science was a direct consequence of his specific definition of science as being constituted by what may be falsifiable. In other words, Popper defined science in terms which necessarily led to the exclusion of psychoanalysis. Thus, defining science in another way may lead to including psychoanalysis into this domain of [[knowledge]].
+
The gist of these early findings (from the 1970s) has been significantly extended in the 1980s and 1990s by further research showing that outside of conscious awareness individuals not only acquire information about frequencies ("occurrences" of features or events) but also co-occurrences (correlations or, technically speaking, covariations) between features or events. Extensive research on non-conscious acquisition of information about co-variations was conducted by [[Pawel Lewicki]], followed by research of D. L. Schachter (who is known for introducing the concept of [[implicit memory]]), L. R. Squire, and others.
  
Still, many, perhaps most, psychologists and cognitive scientists agree that many things of which we are not conscious happen in our mind(s).
+
In the learning phase of a typical study, participants were exposed to a stream of stimuli (trials or events, such as strings of letters, digits, pictures, or descriptions of stimulus persons) containing some consistent but non-salient (hidden) co-variation between features or events. For example, every stimulus a person presented as "fair" would also have a slightly elongated face. It turned out that even if the manipulated co-variations were non-salient and inaccessible to subjects' conscious awareness, the perceivers would still acquire a non-conscious working knowledge about those co-variations. For example, if in the testing phase of the study, participants were asked to make intuitive judgments about the personalities of new stimulus persons presented only as pictures (with no personality descriptions), and judge the "fairness" of the depicted individuals, they tend to follow the rules non-consciously acquired in the learning phase and if the stimulus person had a slightly elongated face, they would report an intuitive feeling that this person was "fair."
  
John Watson criticizes the idea of an "unconscious mind," because he wanted scientists to focus on observable behaviors, seen from the outside, rather than on introspection. Karl Popper objected not so much to the idea that things happened in our minds that we are unconscious of; he objected to investigations of mind that were not falsifiable. If Freud could connect every imaginable experimental outcome with his theory of the unconscious mind, then no experiment can refute his theory.
+
A non-conscious acquisition of information about co-variations appears to be one of the fundamental and ubiquitous processes involved in the acquisition of knowledge (skills, experience) or even preferences or [[personality]] dispositions, including disorders or symptoms of disorders.
  
The argument seems to be about ''how'' mind will be studied, not whether there is anything that happens unconsciously or not.
+
===A note on terminology: "unconscious" vs. "non-conscious"===
 +
Unlike in the psychoanalytic research tradition that uses the terms "unconscious" or "subconscious," in the cognitive tradition, the processes that are not mediated by conscious awareness are sometimes referred to as "non-conscious." This term (rarely used in psychoanalysis) stresses the empirical and purely descriptive nature of that phenomenon (a qualification as simply "not being conscious") in the tradition of cognitive research.
  
 +
Specifically, the process is non-conscious when even highly motivated individuals fail to report it. Few theoretical assumptions are made about the process, unlike in [[psychoanalysis]] where, for example, it is postulated that some of these processes are being repressed in order to achieve certain goals.
  
 
==See also==
 
==See also==
 
 
* [[Carl Jung]]'s concept of a [[collective unconscious]]
 
* [[Carl Jung]]'s concept of a [[collective unconscious]]
* [[Jacques Lacan]]'s assertion that "the unconscious is structured like a language".
+
* [[Jacques Lacan]]'s assertion that "the unconscious is structured like a language."
* [[consciousness]]
+
* [[Consciousness]]
* [[mind's eye]]
 
* [[transpersonal psychology]]
 
 
* [[Unconscious communication]]
 
* [[Unconscious communication]]
 
* [[Psychology of religion]]
 
* [[Psychology of religion]]
 +
* [[Dream]]
 +
* [[Subconscious mind]]
 +
* [[Humanistic psychology]]
 +
* [[Philosophy of mind]]
 +
 +
==Notes==
 +
{{reflist}}
 +
 +
==References==
 +
*Alexander, Charles N. and Ellen Langer. ''Higher Stages of Human Development: Perspectives on Adult Growth''. Oxford University Press, 1990. ISBN 978-0195034837
 +
*Berger, Peter L. and Thomas Luckmann. ''The Social Construction of Reality: A Treatise in the Sociology of Knowledge''. Anchor, 1967. ISBN 978-0385058988
 +
*Crews, Frederick. ''Unauthorized Freud: Doubters Confront a Legend''. Penguin, 1999. ISBN 978-0140280173
 +
*Ellenberger, Henri. ''Discovery of the Unconscious''. Basic Books, 1970. ISBN 978-0465016723
 +
*Freud, Sigmund. ''The Interpretation of Dreams''. NuVision Publications, [1899] 2007. ISBN 978-1595479365
 +
*Jung, C. ''The Development of Personality (Collected Works of C.G. Jung Vol.17)''. Bollingen Publishers, [1954] 1981. ISBN 0691018383
 +
*Jung, C. G., and A. Jaffe. ''Memories, Dreams, Reflections''. London: Collins, 1962. ISBN 0679723951
 +
*Lacan, Jacques. ''The Seminar, Book II. The Ego in Freud's Theory and in the Technique of Psychoanalysis, 1954-1955'', ed. by Jacques-Alain Miller, transl. by Sylvana Tomaselli. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Co., 1991. ISBN 978-0393307092
 +
*Lacan, Jacques. ''The Seminar XI, The Four Fundamental Concepts of Psychoanalysis'', ed. by Jacques-Alain Miller, transl. by Alan Sheridan. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Co., 1998. ISBN 978-0393317756
 +
*Leary, Mark R. and June Price Tangney. ''Handbook of Self and Identity''. The Guilford Press, 2005. ISBN 978-1593852375
 +
*Roth, Michael. ''Freud: Conflict and Culture: Essays on His Life, Work, and Legacy''. Vintage, 2000. ISBN 978-0679772927
  
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
*[[Donald Olding Hebb|Hebbian]] [http://cogprints.org/1652/00/hebb.html Unconscious]
+
All links retrieved May 2, 2023.
*[http://ist-socrates.berkeley.edu/~kihlstrm/rediscovery.htm The Rediscovery of the Unconscious]
+
 
*[http://cogprints.org/2130/00/dennett-chalmers.htm Unfelt Feelings]
+
*[http://cogprints.org/722/   Nonconscious Acquisition of Information (a reprint from American Psychologist, 1992)]
  
  
{{credit1|Unconscious_mind|49134387}}
+
{{credits|Unconscious_mind|163084770}}

Latest revision as of 01:35, 3 May 2023


Part of a series of articles on
Psychoanalysis
Psychoanalysis

Constructs
Psychosexual development
Psychosocial development
Conscious • Preconscious
Unconscious
Id, ego, and super-ego
Libido • Drive
Transference • Resistance
Defense mechanism

Important Figures
Sigmund FreudCarl Jung
Alfred AdlerOtto Rank
Anna FreudMargaret Mahler
Karen HorneyJacques Lacan
Ronald Fairbairn • Melanie Klein
Harry Stack Sullivan
Erik Erikson • Nancy Chodorow

Schools of Thought
Self psychology • Lacanian
Analytical psychology
Object relations
Interpersonal • Relational
Attachment • Ego psychology

Psychology Portal


The Unconscious is a rich concept with a multi-faceted history. For Freud it began as that part of the mind that contained our repressed anxieties, and later it developed into the site of repression for the Oedipus complex, which is the illicit desire to sleep with one's mother and kill one's father. Still later, when Freud developed his structural model, it became the site not only of the Id, but also the Superego, which is the civilizing "instinct" that represented the legacy of the parental voice, making both inaccessible to the functioning Ego.

For Lacan the unconscious was "structured like a language," and in reality it was language, that is, that element which is already given and is not really available to subjectivity. For Jung the unconscious contains both personal material that has been repressed or simply forgotten, but more importantly it contains the collective unconscious, an accumulation of inherited experiences of all humankind that guides and advises our conscious mind. For cognitive psychologists it consists of processes and information that operate, without need for our conscious intervention, to enable us to make sense of the world.

Many others reject the whole notion of an unconscious mind, regarding it as merely a social construction, denying the need to invoke mental processes that are not accessible, and arguing against the validity of such non-falsifiable theories. Still, various observers throughout history have argued that there are influences on consciousness from other parts of the mind, invoking notions such as intuition. Other terms that relate to semi-conscious states or processes include: awakening, implicit memory, subliminal messages, trance, and hypnosis. While sleep, sleep walking, delirium, and coma may signal the presence of unconscious processes they may be different from an unconscious mind.

Those who acknowledge the spiritual nature of human beings note that spiritual senses permit people to communicate with the spiritual world, providing access to information and processes that can be understood as a deeper level of each mind. However, for many people their spiritual senses are so dulled that they are generally unaware of them and their "spiritual mind," thus it has been relegated as the role of the unconscious.

Historical overview

Ancient roots

The idea of an unconscious mind originated in antiquity [1] and has been explored across cultures. It was recorded between 2500 and 600 B.C.E. in the Hindu texts known as the Vedas, found today in Ayurvedic medicine.[2] [3] [4] [5] In the Vedic worldview, consciousness is the basis of physiology [6] [7] and pure consciousness is "an abstract, silent, completely unified field of consciousness" [8] within "an architecture of increasingly abstract, functionally integrated faculties or levels of mind." [9]

Literary roots

William Shakespeare explored the role of the unconscious [10] in many of his plays, without naming it as such. [11] [12] [13] In the nineteenth century Gothic fiction also treated the unconscious mind in such works as Robert Louis Stevenson's Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde.

The unconscious in philosophy

Western philosophers, such as Spinoza, Leibniz, Schopenhauer, and Nietzsche, developed a western view of mind which foreshadowed those of Freud's thought. Schopenhauer was also influenced by his reading of the Vedas and the Jewish mysticism of the Kabbalah. Freud drew on his own Jewish roots to develop an interpersonal examination of the unconscious mind [14] [15] [16] as well as his own therapeutic roots in hypnosis into an apparently new therapeutic intervention and its associated rationale, known as psychoanalysis.

Articulating the idea of something not conscious or actively denied to awareness with the symbolic constructs of language has been a process of human thought and interpersonal influence for over a thousand years. Freud and his followers popularized unconscious motivation in a culture of the individual and within a philosophical tradition that emphasized the Subject, which posited a self viewed as both separate and sufficient.

The resultant status of the unconscious mind may be viewed as a social construction–that the unconscious exists because people agree to behave as if it exists. [17] Symbolic interactionism discusses this further and argues that people's selves (conscious and unconscious) are although purposeful and creative are nevertheless social products. [18]

Unconscious process and unconscious mind

Neuroscience, while an unlikely place to find support for a proposition as adaptable as the unconscious mind, [19] has nonetheless produced some interesting results. "Researchers at Columbia University Medical Center have found that fleeting images of fearful faces—images that appear and disappear so quickly that they escape conscious awareness—produce unconscious anxiety that can be detected in the brain with the latest neuroimaging machines."[20] The conscious mind is hundreds of milliseconds behind those unconscious processes.

While these results represent research into the unconscious processes of the mind, a distinction has to be drawn between unconscious processes and the unconscious mind. They are not identical. The results of neuroscience cannot demonstrate the existence of the unconscious mind. The unconscious mind and its expected psychoanalytic contents [21] [22] [23] [24] [25] [26] are also different from unconsciousness, coma, and a minimally conscious state. Psychoanalytic theory is, at best, a metanarrative on the way the mind functions, and not the result of scientific findings.[27]

The psychoanalytic unconscious

Structure of the unconscious

The term "unconscious mind" is most closely associated with Sigmund Freud and his followers. It lies at the heart of psychoanalysis.

Consciousness, in Freud's topographical view (which was his first of several psychological models of the mind) was a relatively thin perceptual aspect of the mind, whereas the subconscious was that merely autonomic function of the brain. The unconscious was considered by Freud throughout the evolution of his psychoanalytic theory a sentient force of will influenced by human drive and yet operating well below the perceptual conscious mind. For Freud, the unconscious is the storehouse of instinctual desires, needs, and psychic actions. While past thoughts and memories may be deleted from immediate consciousness, they direct the thoughts and feelings of the individual from the realm of the unconscious. In this early view, the psychic struggle exists between the instinctual forces of the unconscious against the social demands of the conscious mind.

In this theory, the unconscious refers to that part of mental functioning of which subjects make themselves unaware. [28]

Freud proposed a vertical and hierarchical architecture of human consciousness: the conscious mind, the preconscious, and the unconscious mind–each lying beneath the other. He believed that significant psychic events take place "below the surface" in the unconscious mind.[29], like hidden messages from the unconscious–a form of intrapersonal communication out of awareness. He interpreted these dream events as both symbolic and actual significance.

In his later structural theory, as a response to the development of the ego theories of his former protégés like Carl Jung and Alfred Adler, Freud divided the mind into the conscious mind or ego and two parts of the Unconscious: the id or instincts and the superego. In this later construct, the unconscious part of the mind was expanded to include not only the instinctual desire of the id, but also the superego which represents the legacy of parental conditioning. In this model, the ego is mediator between id and superego.

The meaning of the unconscious

In Freud's earlier model, the unconscious does not include all that is not conscious, but rather only what is actively repressed from conscious thought or what the person is averse to knowing consciously. That is, the part of the unconscious that is in conflict with conscious awareness. For Freud, the unconscious was a repository for socially unacceptable ideas, wishes or desires, traumatic memories, and painful emotions put out of mind by the mechanism of psychological repression. However, the contents did not necessarily have to be solely negative. In the psychoanalytic view, the unconscious is a force that can only be recognized by its effects–it expresses itself in the symptom.

In a sense, this view places the self in relationship to their unconscious as an adversary, warring with itself to keep what is unconscious hidden. The therapist is then a mediator trying to allow the unspoken or unspeakable to reveal itself using the tools of psychoanalysis. Messages arising from a conflict between conscious and unconscious are likely to be cryptic, in the form of slips of the tongue or symptoms that require decoding. The psychoanalyst is presented as an expert in interpreting those messages. Unconscious thoughts are not directly accessible to ordinary introspection, but are supposed to be capable of being "tapped" and "interpreted" by special methods and techniques such as random association, dream analysis, and verbal slips (commonly known as a Freudian slip), examined and conducted during psychoanalysis.

This model was problematized by the structural theory, which viewed the superego as another element of the unconscious. In this view, the ego is a staging ground for the battle between the unsocial, even anti-social, demands of the id and the superego, representing the parental, social conscience. On adopting this model, Freud began to eschew talk of a "psychoanalytic cure." The role of the analyst remained to make what was unconscious conscious, but Freud realized that the result of this knowledge would not be a cure.

Post-Freudian unconscious

Freud's theory of the unconscious was substantially transformed by some of his followers, notable among them Carl Jung and Jacques Lacan.

Jung's collective unconscious

Carl Jung developed his unconscious concept in an entirely different direction than Freud. He divided the unconscious into two parts: the personal unconscious and the collective unconscious. The personal unconscious is a reservoir of material that was once conscious but has been forgotten or suppressed. This material is unique to the individual, a product of their personality and experience. There is a considerable two way traffic between the ego and the personal unconscious. For example, our attention can wander from this printed page to a memory of something we did yesterday.

The collective unconscious is the deepest level of the psyche containing the accumulation of inherited experiences. It is common to everyone. The collective unconscious has a better sense of the self ideal than the ego or conscious self has, and thus directs the self, via archetypes, dreams, and intuition, to self-actualization. Jung called the unconscious the great friend, guide, and adviser of the conscious.

Lacan's linguistic unconscious

Main article: Jacques Lacan

Jacques Lacan's psychoanalytic theory, based on the rise of modern Structuralism, contends that the unconscious is structured like a language.

The unconscious, Lacan argued, was not a more primitive or archetypal part of the mind separate from the conscious, linguistic ego, but rather, a formation every bit as complex and linguistically sophisticated as consciousness itself.

Lacan argues that if the unconscious is structured like a language, then the self is denied any point of reference to which to be 'restored' following trauma or 'identity crisis'. In this way, Lacan's thesis of the structurally dynamic unconscious is also a challenge to the ego psychology of Anna Freud and her American followers.

Lacan's theory is based on the structural linguistics of Ferdinand de Saussure and Roman Jakobson, based on the function of the signifier and signified in signifying chains. This has left Lacan's model of mental functioning open to severe critique, since in mainstream linguistics Saussurean models have largely been replaced.

The starting point for the linguistic theory of the unconscious was a re-reading of Freud's The Interpretation of Dreams. There, Freud identifies two mechanisms at work in the formation of unconscious fantasies: condensation and displacement. Under Lacan's linguistic reading, condensation is identified with the linguistic trope of metonymy, and displacement with metaphor.

Lacan applied the ideas of de Saussure and Jakobson to psychoanalytic practice. However, while de Saussure described the linguistic sign as a relationship between a signified and an arbitrary signifier, Lacan inverted the relationship, putting in first place the signifier as determining the signified, making it closer to Freud's position that human beings know what they say only as a result of a chain of signifiers, a-posteriori. Lacan began this work with Freud's case study of Emma (1895), whose symptoms were disenchained in a two-phase temporal process. Lacan's approach brought Freud in greater proximity to the structuralist and post-structuralist theories of modernity. For Lacan, modernity is the era when humans begin to grasp their essential dependence on language.

Controversy

Today, there are still fundamental disagreements within psychology about the nature of the unconscious mind. Outside formal psychology, a whole world of pop-psychological speculation has grown up in which the unconscious mind is held to have any number of properties and abilities, from animalistic and innocent, child-like aspects to savant-like, all-perceiving, mystical and occultic properties.

The unconscious may simply stand as a metaphor that ought not to be taken literally. There is a great controversy over the concept of an unconscious in regard to its scientific or rational validity and whether the unconscious mind exists at all. Among philosophers, is Karl Popper, one of Freud's most notable contemporary opponents. Popper argued that Freud's theory of the unconscious was not falsifiable, and therefore not scientific. He objected not so much to the idea that things happened in our minds that we are unconscious of; he objected to investigations of mind that were not falsifiable. If one could connect every imaginable experimental outcome with Freud's theory of the unconscious mind, then no experiment could refute the theory.

Unlike Popper, the epistemologist Adolf Grunbaum has argued that psychoanalysis could be falsifiable, but its evidence has serious epistemological problems. David Holmes [30] examined sixty years of research about the Freudian concept of “repression,” and concluded that there is no positive evidence for this concept. Given the lack of evidence of many Freudian hypotheses, some scientific researchers proposed the existence of unconscious mechanisms that are very different from the Freudian ones. They speak of a “cognitive unconscious” (John Kihlstrom), [31] [32] an “adaptive unconscious” (Timothy Wilson), [33] or a “dumb unconscious” (Loftus & Klinger) [34] that executes automatic processes but lacks the complex mechanisms of repression and symbolic return of the repressed.

Ludwig Wittgenstein and Jacques Bouveresse argued that Freudian thought exhibits a systemic confusion between reasons and causes; the method of interpretation can give reasons for new meanings, but are useless to find causal relations (which require experimental research). Wittgenstein gave the following example (in his Conversations with Rush Rhees), "if we throw objects on a table, and we give free associations and interpretations about those objects, we’ll find a meaning for each object and its place, but we won’t find the causes."

In the social sciences, John Watson, who is considered the first American behaviorist, criticized the idea of an "unconscious mind," along similar lines of reasoning, and focused on observable behaviors rather than on introspection. Other early psychologists, such as the experimental psycholgist Wilhelm Wundt, regarded psychology as the scientific study of immediate experience, and thus the study of human consciousness, or the mind, as long as mind is understood as the totality of conscious experience at a given moment. Wundt denied the role of unconscious processes, defining psychology as the study of conscious, and therefore observable, states.

Other critics of Freudian unconscious were Hans Eysenck, Jacques Van Rillaer, Frank Cioffi, Marshal Edelson, and Edward Erwin. Some stress, however, that these critics did not grasp the real importance of Freud conceptions, and instead tried to criticize Freud on the basis of other fields.

In modern cognitive psychology, many researchers have sought to strip the notion of the unconscious from its Freudian heritage, and alternative terms such as 'implicit' or 'automatic' have come into currency. These traditions emphasize the degree to which cognitive processing happens outside the scope of cognitive awareness and how what we are unaware of can influence other cognitive processes as well as behavior. [35] [36] [37] [38] [39] Active research traditions related to the unconscious include implicit memory (for example, priming or attitude) and non-conscious acquisition of knowledge (such as work by Pawel Lewicki).

Unconscious mind in contemporary cognitive psychology

Research

While historically the psychoanalytic research tradition was the first to focus on the phenomenon of unconscious mental activity (and still the term "unconsciousness" or "the subconscious," for many, appears to be not only deeply rooted in, but almost synonymous with psychoanalytic tradition), there is an extensive body of research in contemporary cognitive psychology devoted to mental activity that is not mediated by conscious awareness.

Most of the cognitive research on unconscious processes has been done in the mainstream, academic tradition of the information processing paradigm. As opposed to the psychoanalytic tradition, driven by the relatively speculative (that is, empirically unverifiable), theoretical concepts such as Oedipus complex or Electra complex, the cognitive tradition of research on unconscious processes is based on relatively few theoretical assumptions and based on empirical research. Cognitive research has demonstrated that outside of conscious awareness, individuals automatically register and acquire more information than they can experience through their conscious thoughts.

Unconscious processing of information about frequency

Hasher and Zacks demonstrated that outside of conscious awareness and without engaging conscious information processing resources individuals register information about the frequency of events.[40] Moreover, their research demonstrated that perceivers do that unintentionally, regardless of the instructions they receive, and regardless of the information processing goals they have. Interestingly, their ability to unconsciously and relatively accurately tally the frequency of events has appeared to have little or no relation to the individual's age, education, intelligence, or personality. Thus, this ability may represent one of the fundamental building blocks of human orientation in the environment and possibly the acquisition of procedural knowledge and experience.

Artificial grammars

Another line of (non-psychoanalytic) early research on unconscious processes was initiated by Arthur Reber, using so-called "artificial grammar" methodology. That research revealed that individuals exposed to novel words created by complex set of artificial, synthetic "grammatical" rules (such as GKHAH, KHABT, and so forth), quickly develop some sort of a "feel" for that grammar and subsequent working knowledge of that grammar, as demonstrated by their ability to differentiate between new grammatically "correct" (consistent with the rules) and "incorrect" (inconsistent) words. Interestingly, that ability does not appear to be mediated, or even accompanied by the declarative knowledge of the rules—individuals' ability to articulate how they distinguish between the correct and incorrect words.

Unconscious acquisition of procedural knowledge

The gist of these early findings (from the 1970s) has been significantly extended in the 1980s and 1990s by further research showing that outside of conscious awareness individuals not only acquire information about frequencies ("occurrences" of features or events) but also co-occurrences (correlations or, technically speaking, covariations) between features or events. Extensive research on non-conscious acquisition of information about co-variations was conducted by Pawel Lewicki, followed by research of D. L. Schachter (who is known for introducing the concept of implicit memory), L. R. Squire, and others.

In the learning phase of a typical study, participants were exposed to a stream of stimuli (trials or events, such as strings of letters, digits, pictures, or descriptions of stimulus persons) containing some consistent but non-salient (hidden) co-variation between features or events. For example, every stimulus a person presented as "fair" would also have a slightly elongated face. It turned out that even if the manipulated co-variations were non-salient and inaccessible to subjects' conscious awareness, the perceivers would still acquire a non-conscious working knowledge about those co-variations. For example, if in the testing phase of the study, participants were asked to make intuitive judgments about the personalities of new stimulus persons presented only as pictures (with no personality descriptions), and judge the "fairness" of the depicted individuals, they tend to follow the rules non-consciously acquired in the learning phase and if the stimulus person had a slightly elongated face, they would report an intuitive feeling that this person was "fair."

A non-conscious acquisition of information about co-variations appears to be one of the fundamental and ubiquitous processes involved in the acquisition of knowledge (skills, experience) or even preferences or personality dispositions, including disorders or symptoms of disorders.

A note on terminology: "unconscious" vs. "non-conscious"

Unlike in the psychoanalytic research tradition that uses the terms "unconscious" or "subconscious," in the cognitive tradition, the processes that are not mediated by conscious awareness are sometimes referred to as "non-conscious." This term (rarely used in psychoanalysis) stresses the empirical and purely descriptive nature of that phenomenon (a qualification as simply "not being conscious") in the tradition of cognitive research.

Specifically, the process is non-conscious when even highly motivated individuals fail to report it. Few theoretical assumptions are made about the process, unlike in psychoanalysis where, for example, it is postulated that some of these processes are being repressed in order to achieve certain goals.

See also

Notes

  1. More modern history is detailed in Henri F. Ellenberger's Discovery of the Unconscious (Basic Books 1970)
  2. Alexander, C. N. Growth of Higher Stages of Consciousness: Maharishi's Vedic Psychology of Human Development. C. N. Alexander and E.J. Langer (eds.). Higher Stages of Human Development. Perspectives on Human Growth. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1990.
  3. Meyer-Dinkgräfe, D. Consciousness and the Actor. A Reassessment of Western and Indian Approaches to the Actor's Emotional Involvement from the Perspective of Vedic Psychology. Frankfurt/Main: Peter Lang, 1996.
  4. Haney, W. S. II. Unity in Vedic aesthetics: the self-interacting dynamics of the knower, the known, and the process of knowing. Analecta Husserliana, 233, 295-319, 1991.
  5. Geraldine Coster 'Yoga and Western Psychology: A comparison' 1934.
  6. Wallace, R. K.; Fagan, J. B.; and Pasco, D. S. Vedic physiology. Modern Science and Vedic Science 2 (1): 3-59, 1988.
  7. King, Michael S. Natural Law and the Bhagavad-Gita: The Vedic Concept of Natural Law Ratio Juris 16 (3), 399–415, 2003.
  8. Alexander, Charles N, Robert W. Cranson, Robert W. Boyer, David W. Orme-Johnson. "Transcendental Consciousness: A Fourth State of consciousness beyond Sleep, Dream, and Waking." Sleep and Dream. Sourcebook. Ed. Jayne Gackenbach. New York, London: Garland Publishing Inc., 282-315, 1986.
  9. Alexander Charles N. et al. "Growth of Higher Stages of Consciousness: Maharishi's Vedic Psychology of Human Development." Higher Stages of Human Development. Perspectives on Human Growth. Eds. Charles N. Alexander and Ellen J. Langer. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 286-341, 1990.
  10. The Design Within: Psychoanalytic Approaches to Shakespeare Edited by M. D. Faber. New York, NY: Science House, 1970. An anthology of 33 papers on Shakespearean plays by psychoanalysts and literary critics whose work has been influenced by psychoanalysis.
  11. Meyer-Dinkgräfe, Daniel "Hamlet’s Procrastination: A Parallel to the Bhagavad-Gita," in Hamlet East West, edited by Marta Gibinska and Jerzy Limon. Gdansk: Theatrum Gedanese Foundation, 187-195, 1998.
  12. Meyer-Dinkgräfe, Daniel 'Consciousness and the Actor: A Reassessment of Western and Indian Approaches to the Actor's Emotional Involvement from the Perspec tive of Vedic Psychology.' Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang, (Series 30: Theatre, Film and Television, Vol. 67) 1996.
  13. Yarrow, Ralph 'Identity and Consciousness East and West: the case of Russell Hoban'. Journal of Literature & Aesthetics, Vol. 5, No. 2, 19-26, 1997.
  14. Drob, S. Freud and the Chasidim: Redeeming The Jewish Soul of Psychoanalysis. Jewish Review 3:1, 1989.
  15. Drob, Sanford L. "This is Gold": Freud, Psychotherapy and the Lurianic Kabbalah (HTML), (1998-2006). Retrieved April 4, 2007.
  16. Drob, Sanford L. "Jung and the Kabbalah" (HTML). History of Psychology. May, 1999 Vol 2(2) 102-118. Retrieved April 4, 2007.
  17. Peter L. Berger and Thomas Luckmann: The Social Construction of Reality. New York: Doubleday, 1966.
  18. Blumer, Herbert. "Society as Symbolic Interaction," in Arnold M. Rose: Human Behavior and Social Process: An Interactionist Approach. Houghton-Mifflin, 1999.
  19. Miller, Laurence In search of the unconscious; evidence for some cornerstones of Freudian theory is coming from an unlikely source - basic neuroscience. Psychology Today, December 1, 1986.
  20. Fleeting Images of Fearful Faces Reveal Neurocircuitry of Unconscious Anxiety Retrieved November 19, 2007.
  21. Crews, F.C. (Ed.) Unauthorized Freud: Doubters confront a legend. New York: Viking, 1998.
  22. Kihlstrom, J.F. Psychodynamics and social cognition: Notes on the fusion of psychoanalysis and psychology. Journal of Personality, 62, 681-696, 1994.
  23. Kihlstrom, J.F. The psychological unconscious. In L.R. Pervin & O. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford, 424-442, 1999.
  24. Macmillan, M.B. Freud evaluated: The completed arc. Cambridge, Ma.: MIT Press, 1996.
  25. Roth, M. Freud: Conflict and culture. New York, NY: Knopf, 1998.
  26. Westen, D. The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 333-371, 1998.
  27. Kihlstrom, J.F. Is Freud Still Alive? No, Not Really Retrieved from [1] April 17, 2007. Extract: No empirical evidence supports any specific proposition of psychoanalytic theory, such as the idea that development proceeds through oral, anal, phallic, and genital stages, or that little boys lust after their mothers and hate and fear their fathers. No empirical evidence indicates that psychoanalysis is more effective, or more efficient, than other forms of psychotherapy, such as systematic desensitization or assertiveness training. No empirical evidence indicates the mechanisms by which psychoanalysis achieves its effects, such as they are, are those specifically predicated on the theory, such as transference and catharsis.
  28. Geraskov, Emil Asenov. The internal contradiction and the unconscious sources of activity. The Journal of Psychology November 1, 1994. Abstract: This article is an attempt to give new meaning to well-known experimental studies, analysis of which may allow us to discover unconscious behavior that has so far remained unnoticed by researchers. Those studies confirm many of the statements by Freud, but they also reveal new aspects of the unconscious psychic. The first global psychological concept of the internal contradiction as an unconscious factor influencing human behavior was developed by Sigmund Freud. In his opinion, this contradiction is expressed in the struggle between the biological instincts and the self. Retrieved from [2] April 17, 2007.
  29. For example, dreaming: Freud called dream symbols the "royal road to the unconscious."
  30. List of his publications at [3] Retrieved April 18, 2007.
  31. Kihlstrom, J.F. The unconscious. In V.S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Human Brain, Vol. 4, 635-646. San Diego, CA: Academic, 2002.
  32. Kihlstrom, J.F., J.S. Beer, and S.B. Klein. Self and identity as memory. In M.R. Leary & J. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity. New York: Guilford Press. 68-90, 2002.
  33. Wilson, T. D. Strangers to Ourselves Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious.
  34. Loftus, E. F., and M.R. Klinger. Is the Unconscious Smart or Dumb? American Psychologist, 47(6), 761-765, 1992.
  35. Greenwald, Anthony G., Sean C. Draine, and Richard L. Abrams. Three Cognitive Markers of Unconscious Semantic Activation Science. Vol. 273, 1699-1702. September 20, 1996.
  36. Gaillard, Raphaël. Antoine Del Cul, Lionel Naccache Fabien Vinckier, Laurent Cohen, and Stanislas Dehaene. Nonconscious semantic processing of emotional words modulates conscious access. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. Vol. 103, 7524-7529. May 9, 2006. Retrieved from [4] April 17, 2007.
  37. Kiefer, Markus and Doreen Brendel. Attentional Modulation of Unconscious "Automatic" Processes: Evidence from Event-related Potentials in a Masked Priming Paradigm Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18:184-198 Retrieved from [5] April 17, 2007.
  38. Naccache, L., R. Gaillard, C. Adam, D. Hasboun, S. Clemenceau, M. Baulac, S. Dehaene, and L. Cohen. A direct intracranial record of emotions evoked by subliminal words. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. Vol. 102, 7713-7717. May 24, 2005. Retrieved from [6] April 17, 2007.
  39. Smith, E.R. and J. DeCoster. Dual-Process Models in Social and Cognitive Psychology: Conceptual Integration and Links to Underlying Memory Systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review 4, 108-131, 2000.
  40. Hasher, L., and R.T. Zacks. Automatic processing of fundamental information: The case of frequency of occurrence. American Psychologist, 39, 1372-1388, 1984.

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Alexander, Charles N. and Ellen Langer. Higher Stages of Human Development: Perspectives on Adult Growth. Oxford University Press, 1990. ISBN 978-0195034837
  • Berger, Peter L. and Thomas Luckmann. The Social Construction of Reality: A Treatise in the Sociology of Knowledge. Anchor, 1967. ISBN 978-0385058988
  • Crews, Frederick. Unauthorized Freud: Doubters Confront a Legend. Penguin, 1999. ISBN 978-0140280173
  • Ellenberger, Henri. Discovery of the Unconscious. Basic Books, 1970. ISBN 978-0465016723
  • Freud, Sigmund. The Interpretation of Dreams. NuVision Publications, [1899] 2007. ISBN 978-1595479365
  • Jung, C. The Development of Personality (Collected Works of C.G. Jung Vol.17). Bollingen Publishers, [1954] 1981. ISBN 0691018383
  • Jung, C. G., and A. Jaffe. Memories, Dreams, Reflections. London: Collins, 1962. ISBN 0679723951
  • Lacan, Jacques. The Seminar, Book II. The Ego in Freud's Theory and in the Technique of Psychoanalysis, 1954-1955, ed. by Jacques-Alain Miller, transl. by Sylvana Tomaselli. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Co., 1991. ISBN 978-0393307092
  • Lacan, Jacques. The Seminar XI, The Four Fundamental Concepts of Psychoanalysis, ed. by Jacques-Alain Miller, transl. by Alan Sheridan. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Co., 1998. ISBN 978-0393317756
  • Leary, Mark R. and June Price Tangney. Handbook of Self and Identity. The Guilford Press, 2005. ISBN 978-1593852375
  • Roth, Michael. Freud: Conflict and Culture: Essays on His Life, Work, and Legacy. Vintage, 2000. ISBN 978-0679772927

External links

All links retrieved May 2, 2023.


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.