Sapir, Edward

From New World Encyclopedia
 
(21 intermediate revisions by 7 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
[[Category:Anthropology]]
+
[[Category:Anthropologists]]
[[Category:Linguistics]]
+
[[Category:Linguists and lexicographers]]
[[Category:Biography]]
 
{{epname}}
 
  
'''Edward Sapir''' (born January 26, 1884 – died February 4, 1939) was an [[United States|American]] [[anthropologist]]-[[linguist]], a leader in American [[structuralism|structural linguistics]], and one of the creators of what is now called the [[Sapir-Whorf hypothesis]]. He is arguably the most influential figure in American linguistics, influencing even [[Noam Chomsky]].
+
{{epname|Sapir, Edward}}
 +
{{Paid}}{{Approved}}{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}{{copyedited}}
 +
'''Edward Sapir''' (January 26, 1884 – February 4, 1939) was an [[United States|American]] [[anthropology|anthropologist]]-[[linguistics|linguist]], a leader in American [[structuralism|structural linguistics]]. He is arguably the most influential figure in American linguistics. His anthropological approach, influenced by [[Franz Boas]], began with his research on [[Native American]]s in an attempt to document their languages before they disappeared. This led to his interest in the inter-connections among [[language]], [[thinking]], and [[culture]]. Work with [[Benjamin Whorf]] in this area led to what became known as the "[[Sapir-Whorf hypothesis]]" and the development of the field of [[ethnolinguistics]]. Sapir's belief that language shapes [[perception]] and influences behavior led him to research the nature of an international auxiliary language. In order to function as a harmonious global society, effective [[communication]] must be achieved, and Sapir's work was instrumental in developing the understanding of how people may communicate without misunderstanding.  
 +
{{toc}}
 +
==Life==
  
==Life==
+
Edward Sapir was born on January 26, 1884 in Lauenburg, [[Prussia]], (now [[Lębork]] in [[Poland]]) to an orthodox [[Judaism|Jewish]] family of [[Lithuania]]n origins. His parents, Jacob David and Eva Seagal Sapir spoke [[Yiddish]] amongst themselves, so Edward learned both German and the language of his parents. When he was six years old his family immigrated to the [[United States]], and eventually settled on the Lower East Side of [[New York City]]. After the death of his younger brother Max, and numerous problems, Sapir’s parents [[divorce]]d in 1910. 
  
'''Edward Sapir''' was born in Lauenburg, [[Prussia]], (now [[Lębork]] in [[Poland]]) to an orthodox Jewish family of Lithuanian origins. His parents, Jacob David and Eva Seagal Sapir spoke Yiddish amongst themselves, so Edward learned both German and the language of his parents. When he was six years old his family immigrated to United States, and eventually settled on the Lower East Side of New York City. After a death of his younger brother Max and numerous problems, Sapir’s parents divorced in 1910.
+
Sapir entered [[Columbia University]] in 1901, on a Pulitzer scholarship he had won three years earlier. He was an extremely bright young man, and many professors saw great potential in him. At Columbia he studied German [[philology]] and Indo-European [[linguistics]]. In 1904, he graduated with a B.A. degree, one year earlier than normal. In 1905, he received his M.A., also in German. However, Sapir’s linguistic interests proved to be much broader. In the next two years he took up projects studying the [[Chinookan|Wishram]] and [[Takelma language|Takelma]] languages. He earned his Ph.D. in [[anthropology]] in 1909, with a dissertation on the Takelma language of southwestern Oregon.
  
Sapir entered Columbia University in 1901, on a Pulitzer scholarship he won three years earlier. He was an extremely bright young man, and many professors saw great potential in him. At Columbia he studied German philology and Indo-European linguistics. In 1904 he graduated with a B.A. degree, one year earlier than normal. In 1905 he received his M.A., also in German. However, Sapir’s linguistic interests proved to be much broader. In the next two years he took up projects studying the [[Chinookan|Wishram]] and [[Takelma language|Takelma]] languages. He earned his Ph.D. in anthropology in 1909 with a dissertation on the Takelma language of southwestern Oregon.
+
While at Columbia, Sapir met his mentor, anthropologist [[Franz Boas]], who was probably the person who provided the initial impetus for Sapir's study of American languages. Boas invoked in Sapir an urgency to study and record endangered languages before they become lost forever. Boas arranged Sapir's employment in 1907-1908 researching the nearly extinct [[Yahi|Yana]] language of northern California.  
  
While at Columbia Sapir met his mentor, anthropologist [[Franz Boas]], who was probably the person who provided the most initial impetus for Sapir's study of American languages. Boas invoked the urgency in Sapir to study and record endangered languages before they become lost forever. Boas arranged Sapir's employment in 1907-08 researching the nearly extinct [[Yahi|Yana]] language of northern California.  
+
Sapir’s first professional appointment was in 1907 at the [[University of California]], Berkeley, after which he moved in 1908 to the [[University of Pennsylvania]], where he taught until 1910. At the same time he conducted several field trips and studied the [[Ute]], Southern Paiute, and [[Hopi]] languages. In 1915, Sapir continued to work with Ishi, the last surviving monolingual speaker of Yahi, the language of the southern [[Yana]] tribe.  
  
Sapir’s first professional appointment was in 1907 at the University of California, Barkley, after which he moved in 1908 to the University of Pennsylvania, where he taught until 1910. At the same time he conducted several field trips and studied Ute language, Southern Paiute, and Hopi. In 1915 Sapir continued to work with [[Ishi]], the monolingual last surviving speaker of [[Yahi]] - (southern Yana) language.  
+
In the years 1910-1925 he built and directed the Anthropological Division in the Geological Survey of Canada, in Ottawa. When he was first hired, he and Marius Barbeau were the first two, full-time anthropologists in [[Canada]]. Among the many accomplishments of this very productive period were a substantial series of publications on Nootka and other languages, and his seminal book ''Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech'' (1921/1955), still important today and eminently readable.  
  
In the years 1910-1925 he built and directed the Anthropological Division in the Geological Survey of Canada, in Ottawa. When he was first hired, he and [[Marius Barbeau]] were the only two, and the first two, full-time anthropologists in Canada. Among the many accomplishments of this very productive period are a substantial series of publications on Nootka and other languages, and his seminal book ''Language'' (1921), still important today and eminently readable..  
+
Sapir married Florence Delson in 1910, but the marriage suffered from the very beginning. Florence developed series of mental and physical ailments and eventually died in 1924. Sapir’s mother helped in raising his three children. During those depressing years Sapir dove into his inner self, starting to write [[poetry]], compose [[music]], and study [[psychology]]. However, he missed research and [[teaching]], which he could not do in Canada.  
  
Sapir married in 1910 to Florence Delson, but the marriage suffered from the very beginning. Florence developed series of mental and physical ailments and eventually died in 1924. Sapir’s mother helped in raising his three children. During those depressing years Sapir dove into his inner self, starting to write poetry, compose music, and study psychology. He missed research and teaching, which he could not do in Canada. Then in 1925 he received a call from the University of Chicago.
+
In 1925, he received a call from the [[University of Chicago]], one of very few research universities at that time in the United States. Before leaving for the teaching position in Chicago, he enabled [[Leonard Bloomfield]] to obtain support from Ottawa for fieldwork on the [[Cree]], essential to his project of historical reconstruction of [[Algonquian languages]].  
  
As he was leaving for a teaching position at the [[University of Chicago]], one of very few research universities then in the United States, he enabled [[Leonard Bloomfield]] to obtain support from Ottawa to do fieldwork on Cree, essential to his project of historical reconstruction in Algonkian. In Chicago Sapir could satisfy his intellectual hunger. His collaboration with interactional psychiatrist Harry Stack Sullivan and political scientist Harold D. Lasswell was particularly notable. Sapir’s sphere of interest widened from solely linguistics to cultural studies, psychology, politics, methodology, etc. He remarried in 1927, to Jean McClenaghan, who was a student at the university at the time. They had two children.
+
In Chicago, Sapir could satisfy his intellectual hunger. His collaboration with interactional psychiatrist [[Harry Stack Sullivan]] and political scientist [[Harold D. Lasswell]] was particularly notable. Sapir’s sphere of interest widened from solely linguistics to include cultural studies, psychology, politics, and methodology. He remarried in 1927, to Jean McClenaghan, who was a student at the university at the time. They had two children.
  
From 1931 to his death Sapir spent at the [[Yale University]], where he became the head of the Department of Anthropology. He also opened there the first Yale school of linguistics. However, he also encountered numerous problems in his work. The economic effects of Depression which limited funding and the anti-Semitic currents at Yale were among the most obvious ones. Sapir’s health suffered the most.  
+
From 1931 to his death, Sapir spent his time at [[Yale University]], where he became head of the department of anthropology. There, he opened the first Yale school of linguistics. However, he also encountered numerous problems in his work. The economic effects of the [[Great Depression]] which limited funding, and the [[anti-Semitism|anti-Semitic]] currents at Yale, were among the most obvious ones. Sapir’s health suffered greatly.  
  
 
Sapir died of heart problems in 1939, at age 55.
 
Sapir died of heart problems in 1939, at age 55.
Line 29: Line 31:
 
==Work==
 
==Work==
  
Sapir’s special focus among American languages was on the [[Athabaskan]] languages, a family he was especially fascinated by: "Dene is probably the son-of-a-bitchiest language in America to actually ''know''...most fascinating of all languages ever invented" (Krauss 1986:157). Among the languages and cultures studied by Sapir are [[Chinookan|Wishram Chinook]], [[Navajo language|Navajo]], [[Nootka]], [[Paiute]], [[Takelma language|Takelma]], and [[Yahi|Yana]].  Although noted for his work on American linguistics, he was also prolific in linguistics in general, as depicted by his book ''Language'', which provides everything from a grammar-typological classification of languages (with examples ranging from [[Chinese language|Chinese]] to [[Nuu-chah-nulth language|Nootka]]) to speculation on the phenomenon on language drift and the arbitrariness of associations between language, race, and culture.  He was also at least a minor participant in the [[international auxiliary language]] movement. Sapir argued for the benefits of a regular grammar and advocated a critical focus on the fundamentals of language unbiased by the idiosyncrasies of national languages in the choice of an international auxiliary language.
+
Sapir’s special focus among [[Native American]] languages was on the [[Athabaskan]] languages, a family he was especially fascinated by: "Dene is probably the …most fascinating of all languages ever invented" (Krauss 1986:157). Other languages and [[culture]]s studied by Sapir include [[Wishram Chinook]], [[Navajo]], [[Nootka]], [[Paiute]], [[Takelma]], and [[Yana]].   
  
Besides pure linguistics, Sapir was also interested in cultural behaviorism and development of personality. He searched for the connection between personality, verbal expression and socially determined behavior. He saw language as a verbal symbol in human relations. Sapir believed that language shapes human perception and directs our behavior. Understanding one culture thus is impossible without understanding historical development of that culture’s language.
+
Although noted for his work on American linguistics, Sapir was also a prolific writer in [[linguistics]] in general, as depicted by his book ''Language,'' which provides everything from a grammar-typological classification of languages (with examples ranging from [[Chinese language|Chinese]] to Nootka) to speculation on the phenomenon on "language drift" and the arbitrariness of associations between language, race, and culture. He was also at least a minor participant in the international auxiliary language movement. Sapir argued for the benefits of a regular [[grammar]] and advocated a critical focus on the fundamentals of language unbiased by the idiosyncrasies of national languages in the choice of an international auxiliary language.
Sapir thought that language is not static, but that it constantly changes. Sapir called that change The Language Drift. Some parts of language change more quickly, some are much slower. The reality changes, so does the language. But also due to the change in language, the reality changes as well. We think, hear, see, and behave through our language. Language serves as a certain filter through which we experience and interpret reality. Every culture has its own language, or a set of filters through which it predisposes its members to certain kind of experience and thinking. Without language it is difficult to imagine life at all.  
 
  
Some suggestions of Sapir about the influence of language on the ways in which people think were adopted and developed by Whorf, initially while he was substitute teaching in the classroom during Sapir's illness. During the 1940s and later this became known as the [[Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis]].  
+
Besides pure linguistics, Sapir was also interested in cultural behaviorism and the development of [[personality]]. He searched for the connection between personality, verbal expression, and socially determined behavior. He saw language as a verbal [[symbol]] in human relations. Sapir believed that language shapes human [[perception]] and directs human behavior. From his view, understanding a culture was impossible without understanding the historical development of that culture’s language.  
  
==Legacy==
+
Sapir argued that language is not static, but that it constantly changes. Sapir called that change the "Language Drift." Some parts of language change quickly while some are much slower. As reality changes, so does the language. Conversely, due to the change in language, reality changes as well. We think, hear, see, and behave through our language. Language serves as a certain filter through which we experience and interpret reality. Every culture has its own language, or set of filters through which it predisposes its members to certain kinds of experience and [[thinking]]. Without language, it is difficult to imagine human life at all.
  
Sapir was one of the first who explored the relations between language and anthropology.  His students include [[Li Fanggui|Fang-kuei Li]], [[Benjamin Whorf]], [[Mary Haas]], and [[Harry Hoijer]], but it was one not formally his student who he came to regard as his intellectual heir, a young [[Semitic Languages|Semiticist]] named [[Zellig Harris]] (who for a time dated his daughter).
+
Some of Sapir's ideas about the influence of language on the ways in which people think were adopted and developed by [[Benjamin Whorf]], initially while he was substitute teaching in the classroom during Sapir's illness. Whorf's development of these ideas later became known as the "Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis," and laid the foundation for [[ethnolinguistics]] to develop as an academic field.
  
Sapir was extremely adored scientist. His way of expression was simple and easy to understand, and he had rather modest personality. He above all wanted to pass his love to linguistics and anthropology to others, and abstained from seeking of personal glory. His students thus liked him and, after his death, put numerous collections of their own essays, dedicating them to him.
+
==Legacy==
 
 
==Bibliography==
 
 
 
*Sapir, Edward. 1911. The problem of noun incorporation in American languages. American Anthropology. 13, 250-82
 
 
 
*Sapir, Edward. 1916. [http://spartan.ac.brocku.ca/~lward/Sapir/Sapir_1916.html ''Time Perspective in Aboriginal American Culture: A Study in Method'']. Canada Department of Mines, Geological Survey, Memoir 90. Anthropological Series, 13.
 
 
 
*Sapir, Edward. 1924. The grammarian and his language. ''American Mercury'', 1, 149-155.
 
 
 
*Sapir, Edward. 1925. [http://www.geocities.com/Athens/Forum/5037/sapir.html ''The Function of an International Auxiliary Language'']. ''Romanic Review'', July 1925.
 
 
 
*Sapir, Edward. 1925. Sound patterns in language. ''Language'', 1, 37-51
 
 
 
*Sapir, Edward. 1933. La réalité psychologique des phonèmes [The psychological reality of phonemes]. ''Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique'', 30, 247-265
 
  
*Sapir, Edward. 1944. Grading, a study in Semantics. ''Philosophy of Science'', 11(2), 93-116
+
Edward Sapir was one of the first who explored the relationship between [[language]] and [[anthropology]]. His students include Li Fanggui, [[Benjamin Whorf]], Mary Haas, and Harry Hoijer. However, it was not one of his formal students whom he came to regard as his intellectual heir, but rather a young scholar of Semitic languages named [[Zellig S. Harris]], (who for a time dated his daughter).
  
*Sapir, Edward. 1955. (original published in 1921). Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech. Harvest Books. ISBN 015-648-2339
+
Sapir was an extremely well-liked scientist. His way of expression was simple and easy to understand, and he had a rather modest personality. Above all, he wanted to pass on his love of [[linguistics]] and anthropology to others. He abstained from seeking personal glory. His students liked him and, after his death, prepared numerous collections of their own essays, dedicating them to him.
  
*Sapir, Edward (Ed.). 1974. (original published in 1939). ''Nootka texts: Tales and Ethnological Narratives with Grammatical Notes and Lexical Materials''. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404118933
+
==Publications==
  
*Sapir, Edward. 1974. (original published in 1909). ''Wishram Texts''. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404581528
+
*Sapir, Edward. 1974 (original 1909). ''Wishram Texts''. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404581528
 
+
*Sapir, Edward. 1911. "The problem of noun incorporation in American languages" in ''American Anthropology''. 13, 250-82.
*Sapir, Edward; Darnell, Regna; Irvine, Judith T.; & Handler, Richard (Eds.). 1999. ''The Collected Works of Edward Sapir: Culture''. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3110126397
+
*Sapir, Edward. 1916. ''Time Perspective in Aboriginal American Culture: A Study in Method''. Canada Department of Mines, Geological Survey, Memoir 90. Anthropological Series, 13.
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1955 (original 1921). ''Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech''. Harvest Books. ISBN 015-648-2339
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1924. "The grammarian and his language" in ''American Mercury''. 1, 149-155.
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1925. "Sound patterns in language" in ''Language''. 1, 37-51.
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1925. [http://www.geocities.com/Athens/Forum/5037/sapir.html "The Function of an International Auxiliary Language"] ''Romanic Review''.
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1933. "La réalité psychologique des phonèmes" (The psychological reality of phonemes) in ''Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique''. 30, 247-265.
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1974 (original 1939). ''Nootka texts: Tales and Ethnological Narratives with Grammatical Notes and Lexical Materials''. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404118933
 +
*Sapir, Edward. 1944. "Grading, a study in Semantics in ''Philosophy of Science''. 11(2), 93-116.
 +
*Sapir, Edward., Darnell, Regna., Irvine, Judith T., & Richard Handler. 1999. ''The Collected Works of Edward Sapir: Culture''. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3110126397
  
 
==References==
 
==References==
  
*Koerner, Konrad & E. F. K. Koerner. (Eds.). 1985. ''Edward Sapir: Appraisals of His Life and Work''.  Philadelphia: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245185
+
*Cowan, William & Michael K. Foster. 1986. ''New perspectives in language, culture, and personality''. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
 
+
*Darnell, Regna. 1989. ''Edward Sapir: Linguist, Anthropologist, Humanist''. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. ISBN 0520066782
*Mandelbaum, David G. (Ed.). 1986. Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture, and Personality. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-01115-5.
+
*Koerner, Konrad & E.F.K. Koerner. 1985. ''Edward Sapir: Appraisals of His Life and Work''.  Philadelphia: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245185
 
+
*Krauss, Michael E. 1986. "Edward Sapir and Athabaskan linguistics, with preliminary annotated bibliography of Sapir's work on Athabaskan and Na-Dene" in ''New perspectives in language, culture, and personality''. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
*Darnell Regna. 1989. ''Edward Sapir: Linguist, Anthropologist, Humanist.''. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. ISBN 0520066782
 
 
 
 
*Lucy, John Arthur. 1992. ''Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis'' (Studies in the Social and Cultural Foundations of Language, No. 12). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.  ISBN 0521387973
 
*Lucy, John Arthur. 1992. ''Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis'' (Studies in the Social and Cultural Foundations of Language, No. 12). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.  ISBN 0521387973
 
+
*Mandelbaum, David G. 1986. ''Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture, and Personality''. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-01115-5.
*Cowan, William & Foster, Michael K. (Eds.). 1986. ''New perspectives in language, culture, and personality''. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
 
 
 
*Michael E. Krauss. 1986. Edward Sapir and Athabaskan linguistics, with preliminary annotated bibliography of Sapir's work on Athabaskan and Na-Dene. In W. Cowan, M. K. Foster, & K. Koerner, New perspectives in language, culture, and personality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
 
  
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
 +
All links retrieved February 12, 2024.
  
*[http://www.bartleby.com/186/ ''Language: An introduction to the study of speech''] Full text  
+
*[http://www.bartleby.com/186/ ''Language: An introduction to the study of speech'']Full text.  
 
 
*[http://www.nap.edu/readingroom/books/biomems/esapir.html Biography of E. Sapir] - National Academy of Sciences
 
 
 
*[http://www.bartleby.com/people/Sapir-Ed.html E. Sapir] – Short biography
 
 
 
*[http://spartan.ac.brocku.ca/~lward/Sapir/Documents.html Selected works] – Some full texts articles by Sapir
 
  
*[http://www.mnsu.edu/emuseum/information/biography/pqrst/sapir_edward.html Edward Sapir: Biography] – Biography
 
  
 
{{Credit1|Edward_Sapir|73661761|}}
 
{{Credit1|Edward_Sapir|73661761|}}

Latest revision as of 23:51, 12 February 2024


Edward Sapir (January 26, 1884 – February 4, 1939) was an American anthropologist-linguist, a leader in American structural linguistics. He is arguably the most influential figure in American linguistics. His anthropological approach, influenced by Franz Boas, began with his research on Native Americans in an attempt to document their languages before they disappeared. This led to his interest in the inter-connections among language, thinking, and culture. Work with Benjamin Whorf in this area led to what became known as the "Sapir-Whorf hypothesis" and the development of the field of ethnolinguistics. Sapir's belief that language shapes perception and influences behavior led him to research the nature of an international auxiliary language. In order to function as a harmonious global society, effective communication must be achieved, and Sapir's work was instrumental in developing the understanding of how people may communicate without misunderstanding.

Life

Edward Sapir was born on January 26, 1884 in Lauenburg, Prussia, (now Lębork in Poland) to an orthodox Jewish family of Lithuanian origins. His parents, Jacob David and Eva Seagal Sapir spoke Yiddish amongst themselves, so Edward learned both German and the language of his parents. When he was six years old his family immigrated to the United States, and eventually settled on the Lower East Side of New York City. After the death of his younger brother Max, and numerous problems, Sapir’s parents divorced in 1910.

Sapir entered Columbia University in 1901, on a Pulitzer scholarship he had won three years earlier. He was an extremely bright young man, and many professors saw great potential in him. At Columbia he studied German philology and Indo-European linguistics. In 1904, he graduated with a B.A. degree, one year earlier than normal. In 1905, he received his M.A., also in German. However, Sapir’s linguistic interests proved to be much broader. In the next two years he took up projects studying the Wishram and Takelma languages. He earned his Ph.D. in anthropology in 1909, with a dissertation on the Takelma language of southwestern Oregon.

While at Columbia, Sapir met his mentor, anthropologist Franz Boas, who was probably the person who provided the initial impetus for Sapir's study of American languages. Boas invoked in Sapir an urgency to study and record endangered languages before they become lost forever. Boas arranged Sapir's employment in 1907-1908 researching the nearly extinct Yana language of northern California.

Sapir’s first professional appointment was in 1907 at the University of California, Berkeley, after which he moved in 1908 to the University of Pennsylvania, where he taught until 1910. At the same time he conducted several field trips and studied the Ute, Southern Paiute, and Hopi languages. In 1915, Sapir continued to work with Ishi, the last surviving monolingual speaker of Yahi, the language of the southern Yana tribe.

In the years 1910-1925 he built and directed the Anthropological Division in the Geological Survey of Canada, in Ottawa. When he was first hired, he and Marius Barbeau were the first two, full-time anthropologists in Canada. Among the many accomplishments of this very productive period were a substantial series of publications on Nootka and other languages, and his seminal book Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech (1921/1955), still important today and eminently readable.

Sapir married Florence Delson in 1910, but the marriage suffered from the very beginning. Florence developed series of mental and physical ailments and eventually died in 1924. Sapir’s mother helped in raising his three children. During those depressing years Sapir dove into his inner self, starting to write poetry, compose music, and study psychology. However, he missed research and teaching, which he could not do in Canada.

In 1925, he received a call from the University of Chicago, one of very few research universities at that time in the United States. Before leaving for the teaching position in Chicago, he enabled Leonard Bloomfield to obtain support from Ottawa for fieldwork on the Cree, essential to his project of historical reconstruction of Algonquian languages.

In Chicago, Sapir could satisfy his intellectual hunger. His collaboration with interactional psychiatrist Harry Stack Sullivan and political scientist Harold D. Lasswell was particularly notable. Sapir’s sphere of interest widened from solely linguistics to include cultural studies, psychology, politics, and methodology. He remarried in 1927, to Jean McClenaghan, who was a student at the university at the time. They had two children.

From 1931 to his death, Sapir spent his time at Yale University, where he became head of the department of anthropology. There, he opened the first Yale school of linguistics. However, he also encountered numerous problems in his work. The economic effects of the Great Depression which limited funding, and the anti-Semitic currents at Yale, were among the most obvious ones. Sapir’s health suffered greatly.

Sapir died of heart problems in 1939, at age 55.

Work

Sapir’s special focus among Native American languages was on the Athabaskan languages, a family he was especially fascinated by: "Dene is probably the …most fascinating of all languages ever invented" (Krauss 1986:157). Other languages and cultures studied by Sapir include Wishram Chinook, Navajo, Nootka, Paiute, Takelma, and Yana.

Although noted for his work on American linguistics, Sapir was also a prolific writer in linguistics in general, as depicted by his book Language, which provides everything from a grammar-typological classification of languages (with examples ranging from Chinese to Nootka) to speculation on the phenomenon on "language drift" and the arbitrariness of associations between language, race, and culture. He was also at least a minor participant in the international auxiliary language movement. Sapir argued for the benefits of a regular grammar and advocated a critical focus on the fundamentals of language unbiased by the idiosyncrasies of national languages in the choice of an international auxiliary language.

Besides pure linguistics, Sapir was also interested in cultural behaviorism and the development of personality. He searched for the connection between personality, verbal expression, and socially determined behavior. He saw language as a verbal symbol in human relations. Sapir believed that language shapes human perception and directs human behavior. From his view, understanding a culture was impossible without understanding the historical development of that culture’s language.

Sapir argued that language is not static, but that it constantly changes. Sapir called that change the "Language Drift." Some parts of language change quickly while some are much slower. As reality changes, so does the language. Conversely, due to the change in language, reality changes as well. We think, hear, see, and behave through our language. Language serves as a certain filter through which we experience and interpret reality. Every culture has its own language, or set of filters through which it predisposes its members to certain kinds of experience and thinking. Without language, it is difficult to imagine human life at all.

Some of Sapir's ideas about the influence of language on the ways in which people think were adopted and developed by Benjamin Whorf, initially while he was substitute teaching in the classroom during Sapir's illness. Whorf's development of these ideas later became known as the "Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis," and laid the foundation for ethnolinguistics to develop as an academic field.

Legacy

Edward Sapir was one of the first who explored the relationship between language and anthropology. His students include Li Fanggui, Benjamin Whorf, Mary Haas, and Harry Hoijer. However, it was not one of his formal students whom he came to regard as his intellectual heir, but rather a young scholar of Semitic languages named Zellig S. Harris, (who for a time dated his daughter).

Sapir was an extremely well-liked scientist. His way of expression was simple and easy to understand, and he had a rather modest personality. Above all, he wanted to pass on his love of linguistics and anthropology to others. He abstained from seeking personal glory. His students liked him and, after his death, prepared numerous collections of their own essays, dedicating them to him.

Publications

  • Sapir, Edward. 1974 (original 1909). Wishram Texts. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404581528
  • Sapir, Edward. 1911. "The problem of noun incorporation in American languages" in American Anthropology. 13, 250-82.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1916. Time Perspective in Aboriginal American Culture: A Study in Method. Canada Department of Mines, Geological Survey, Memoir 90. Anthropological Series, 13.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1955 (original 1921). Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech. Harvest Books. ISBN 015-648-2339
  • Sapir, Edward. 1924. "The grammarian and his language" in American Mercury. 1, 149-155.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1925. "Sound patterns in language" in Language. 1, 37-51.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1925. "The Function of an International Auxiliary Language" Romanic Review.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1933. "La réalité psychologique des phonèmes" (The psychological reality of phonemes) in Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique. 30, 247-265.
  • Sapir, Edward. 1974 (original 1939). Nootka texts: Tales and Ethnological Narratives with Grammatical Notes and Lexical Materials. AMS Press Inc. ISBN 0404118933
  • Sapir, Edward. 1944. "Grading, a study in Semantics in Philosophy of Science. 11(2), 93-116.
  • Sapir, Edward., Darnell, Regna., Irvine, Judith T., & Richard Handler. 1999. The Collected Works of Edward Sapir: Culture. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3110126397

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Cowan, William & Michael K. Foster. 1986. New perspectives in language, culture, and personality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
  • Darnell, Regna. 1989. Edward Sapir: Linguist, Anthropologist, Humanist. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. ISBN 0520066782
  • Koerner, Konrad & E.F.K. Koerner. 1985. Edward Sapir: Appraisals of His Life and Work. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245185
  • Krauss, Michael E. 1986. "Edward Sapir and Athabaskan linguistics, with preliminary annotated bibliography of Sapir's work on Athabaskan and Na-Dene" in New perspectives in language, culture, and personality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. ISBN 9027245223
  • Lucy, John Arthur. 1992. Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis (Studies in the Social and Cultural Foundations of Language, No. 12). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0521387973
  • Mandelbaum, David G. 1986. Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture, and Personality. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-01115-5.

External links

All links retrieved February 12, 2024.


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.