Difference between revisions of "Ethology" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
 
(27 intermediate revisions by 8 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Claimed}}{{Contracted}}
+
{{Ebapproved}}{{Approved}}{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}{{Paid}}{{Copyedited}}
[[Image:V31-d-Graugans.JPG|right|thumb|The egg-rolling behavior of the Greylag Goose is a widely cited example of a ''fixed-action pattern'', one of the key concepts used by ethologists to explain animal behavior.]]
+
[[Image:V31-d-Graugans.JPG|right|thumb|250px|The egg-rolling behavior of the greylag goose is a widely cited example of a ''fixed-action pattern,'' one of the key concepts used by ethologists to explain animal behavior.]]
  
'''Ethology''' is a branch of [[zoology]] concerned with the study of [[animal]] [[behavior]]. Ethologists study a range of animal behaviors including sexual selection, social behavior, kinship, reciprocity and cooperation, parental investment, conflict and aggression.
+
'''Ethology''' is a branch of [[zoology]] concerned with the study of [[animal]] [[behavior]]. Ethologists take a comparative approach, studying behaviors ranging from kinship, cooperation, and parental investment, to conflict, sexual selection, and aggression across a variety of [[species]]. Today ''ethology'' as a disciplinary label has largely been replaced by [[behavioral ecology]] and [[evolutionary psychology]]. These rapidly growing fields tend to place greater emphasis on social relationships rather than on the individual animal; however, they retain ethology’s tradition of fieldwork and its grounding in evolutionary theory.
  
Methodologically, ethologists engage in a tradition of hypothesis-driven experimental investigation, often in the field. This combination of lab work with field study reflects an important underpinning of the discipline: behavior is assumed to be adaptive to the species’  natural environment.  
+
The study of animal behavior touches upon the fact that people receive joy from [[nature]] and also typically see themselves in a special role as stewards of creation. Behavior is one aspect of the vast diversity of nature that enhances human enjoyment. People are fascinated with the many behaviors of animals, whether the communication "dance" of [[honeybee]]s, or the hunting behavior of the big [[cat]]s, or the altruistic behavior of a [[dolphin]]. In addition, humans generally see themselves with the responsibility to love and care for nature.  
  
Ethology emerged as a discipline in the 1920s; its founding practitioners, themselves influenced by the German ornithologist [[Oskar Heinroth]], were [[Konrad Lorenz]], [[Karl von Frisch]], and [[Niko Tinbergen]], who were jointly awarded the 1973 [[Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine]] for their contributions to the study of behavior. (maybe construe them as popularizers and synthesizrs of early work of Douglas Spalding, Heinroth, et al.)
+
The study of animal behavior also helps people to understand more about themselves. From an evolutionary point of view, organisms of diverse lineages are related through the process of [[evolution#theory of descent with modification|descent with modification]]. From a religious point of view, human also stand as “microcosms of nature" (Burns 2006). Thus, the understanding of animals helps to better understand ourselves.  
  
One of the key ideas of classical ethology is the concept of [[fixed action pattern]]s (FAPs). FAPs are instinctive responses that will occur reliably in the presence of identifiable stimuli (called ''sign stimuli'' or ''releasing stimuli''). For example, the greylag goose, like similar [[waterfowl]], will roll a displaced [[Egg (biology)|egg]] near its nest back to the others with its beak. The sight of the displaced egg triggers this mechanism. If the egg is taken away, the animal continues to engage in egg-rolling behavior, pulling its head back as if an imaginary egg is still being maneuvered by the underside of its beak. It will also attempt to move other egg-shaped objects, such as a golf ball, door knob, or even an egg too large to have been laid by the goose itself (Tinbergen, 1991).
+
Ethologists engage in hypothesis-driven experimental investigation, often in the field. This combination of lab work with field study reflects an important conceptual underpinning of the discipline: behavior is assumed to be ''adaptive''; in other words, something that makes it better suited in its environment and consequently improves its chances of survival and reproductive success.  
 +
{{toc}}
 +
Ethology emerged as a discrete discipline in the 1920s, through the efforts of [[Konrad Lorenz]], [[Karl von Frisch]], and [[Niko Tinbergen]], who were jointly awarded the 1973 [[Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine]] for their contributions to the study of behavior. They were in turn influenced by the foundational work of, among others, ornithologists [[Oskar Heinroth]] and [[Julian Huxley]] and the American [[myrmecologist]] (study of ants) [[William Morton Wheeler]], who popularized the term ''ethology'' in a seminal 1902 paper.
  
Classical ethology spawned problematic grand theories about internal control mechanisms and the extent to which they were genetically hardwired (''innate'' or ''instinctive'', according to the terminology). (what do we think today that's different - more flexible?)
+
==Important concepts==
 +
One of the key ideas of classical ethology is the concept of [[fixed action pattern]]s (FAPs). FAPs are stereotyped behaviors that occur in a predictable, inflexible sequence in response to an identifiable stimulus from the environment.  
  
Although ''ethology'' as a disciplinary label has largely faded from use, the desire to understand the animal world has made ethology's intellectual inheritors – such as [[behavioral ecology]] and [[evolutionary psychology]] – rapidly growing fields. Today, students of animal behavior tend to place greater emphasis on social relationships (rather than the individual as variable); however, these studies retain ethology’s tradition of hypothesis-driven investigation and evolutionary focus.
+
[[Image:Larus_Dominicanus.jpg|right|thumb|250px|[[Kelp Gull]] chicks peck at a red spot on their mother's beak to stimulate the regurgitating reflex, another example of a fixed action pattern.]]
 +
For example, at the sight of a displaced [[Egg (biology)|egg]] near the nest, the greylag goose ''(Anser anser)'' will roll the egg back to the others with its beak. If the egg is removed, the animal continues to engage in egg-rolling behavior, pulling its head back as if an imaginary egg is still being maneuvered by the underside of its beak. It will also attempt to move other egg-shaped objects, such as a golf ball, doorknob, or even an egg too large to have been laid by the goose itself (Tinbergen 1991).  
  
==Relation to comparative psychology==
+
Another important concept is ''filial imprinting,'' a form of learning that occurs in young animals, usually during a critical, formative period of their lives. During imprinting, a young animal learns to direct some of its social responses to a parent or sibling.
A comparison of classical ethology to early work in [[comparative psychology]], an alternative approach to animal behavior helps to illuminate the methodological and conceptual underpinnings of both:
 
 
 
*As opposed to ethology, comparative psychology construes its study as a branch of [[psychology]] rather than as one of [[biology]]. Thus, where comparative psychology sees the study of animal behavior in the context of what is known about human psychology, ethology situates the study of animal behavior in the context of what is known about animal [[anatomy]], [[physiology]], [[neurobiology]], and [[phylogenetic]] history.
 
* Early comparative psychologists concentrated on gaining extensive knowledge of the behavior of very few [[species]], while ethologists were more interested in gaining knowledge of behavior in a wide range of species in order to be able to make principled comparisons across [[alpha taxonomy|taxonomic]] groups. Comparative psychologists were interested more in similarities than differences in behavior; they were seeking general laws of behavior, especially relating to development, which could be applied to all animal species, including humans.
 
*Ethologists focused primarily on lab experiments involving a handful of species, mainly rats and pigeons, whereas early ethologists concentrated on behavior in natural situations, tending to describe it as instinctive.
 
 
 
The differences have been characterized as study of general processes (psychology) versus a study of adaptive specialization (ethology) (26).
 
  
The two approaches are complementary rather than competitive, but they do lead to different perspectives and sometimes to conflicts of opinion about matters of substance. In addition, for most of the [[twentieth century]], ethology, which had developed in Europe, failed to gain a strong foothold in [[North America]], where comparative psychology was dominant. However, in the 1970s, a rapprochement was achieved, and today, animal behavior is a more integrated discipline, with comparative psychologists and ethological animal behaviorists working on similar problems and publishing side by side in the same journals.
+
Despite its valuable contributions to the study of animal behavior, classical ethology also spawned problematic general theories that viewed even complex behaviors as genetically hardwired (i.e., ''innate'' or ''instinctive''). Models of behavior have since been revised to account for more flexible decision-making processes (Barnard 2003).
  
 
==Methodology==
 
==Methodology==
 
===Tinbergen's four questions for ethologists===
 
===Tinbergen's four questions for ethologists===
(tradition of hypothesis-driven research) Lorenz's collaborator, Niko Tinbergen, argued that ethologists should consider the following four categories when attempting to formulate a hypothesis that explains any instance of behavior:
+
The practice of ethological investigation is rooted in hypothesis-driven experimentation. [[Konrad Lorenz|Lorenz]]'s collaborator, [[Niko Tinbergen]], argued that ethologists should consider the following categories when attempting to formulate a hypothesis that explains any instance of behavior:
 +
 
 +
* Function: How does the behavior impact the animal's chance of survival and reproduction?
 +
* Mechanism: What are the stimuli that elicit the response? How has the response been modified by recent learning?
 +
* Development: How does the behavior change with age? What early experiences are necessary for the behavior to be demonstrated?
 +
* Evolutionary history: How does the behavior compare with similar behavior in related species? How might the behavior have arisen through the evolutionary development of the [[species]], [[genus]], or group?
  
* Function: how does the behavior impact the animal's chance of survival and reproduction?
+
The four questions are meant to be complementary, revealing various facets of the motives underlying a given behavior.
* Mechanism: what are the stimuli that elicit the response? How has the response been modified by recent learning?
 
* Development: how does the behavior change with age? What early experiences are necessary for the behavior to be demonstrated?
 
* Evolutionary history: how does the behavior compare with similar behavior in related species? How might the behavior have arisen through the process of [[phylogeny]] (the development of the species, genus, or group)?
 
  
 
===Using fieldwork to test hypotheses===
 
===Using fieldwork to test hypotheses===
(transition) Other early ethologists, such as [[Oskar Heinroth]] and [[Julian Huxley]], instead concentrated on behaviours that can be called instinctive, or natural, in that they occur in all members of a species under specified circumstances.  Their first step in studying the behavior of a new species was to construct an '''ethogram''' (a description of the main types of natural behavior with their frequencies of occurrence).  This approach provided an objective, cumulative base of data about behaviour, which subsequent researchers could check and build on.
+
As an example of how an ethologist might approach a question about animal behavior, consider the study of [[hearing]] in an [[echolocating]] [[bat]]. A species of bat may use frequency chirps to probe the environment while in flight. A traditional neuroscientific study of the [[auditory system]] of the bat would involve [[Anesthesia|anesthetizing]] it, performing a [[craniotomy]] to insert [[Electrophysiology#Electrophysiology_-_the_techniques|recording electrodes]] in its [[brain]], and then recording [[action potential|neural responses]] to pure [[pitch (music)|tone]] [[stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]] played from loudspeakers. In contrast, an ideal ethological study would attempt to replicate the natural conditions of the animal as closely as possible. It would involve recording from the animal’s brain while it is awake, producing its natural calls while performing a behavior such as insect capture.
 
 
''Neuroethology'' is a branch of [[neuroscience]] that emphasizes the study of neural mechanisms of natural [[behavior]]. As an example, consider the study of [[hearing]] in an [[echolocating bat]]. A species of bat may use frequency chirps to probe the environment while in flight. A traditional neuroscientific study of the [[auditory system]] of the bat would involve [[Anesthesia|anesthetizing]] it, performing a [[craniotomy]] to insert [[Electrophysiology#Electrophysiology_-_the_techniques|recording electrodes]] in its [[brain]], and then recording [[action potential|neural responses]] to pure [[pitch (music)|tone]] [[stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]] played from [[loudspeakers]]. In contrast, an ideal neuroethological study would attempt to replicate the natural conditions of the animal as closely as possible. It would involve recording from the animal’s brain while it is [[awake]], producing its natural calls while performing some natural behavior such as insect capture.
 
  
 
==Key principles and concepts==
 
==Key principles and concepts==
 
===Behaviors are adaptive responses to natural selection===
 
===Behaviors are adaptive responses to natural selection===
Because ethology is understood as a branch of biology, ethologists have been particularly concerned with the [[evolution]] of behavior and the understanding of behavior in terms of the theory of [[natural selection]].  In one sense, the first modern ethologist was [[Charles Darwin]], whose book ''The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals'' (1872) has influenced many ethologists. Although Darwin’s protégé [[George Romanes]] became one of the founders of comparative psychology, positing a similarity of cognitive processes and mechanisms between animals and humans, something on neo-Darwinism.
+
Because ethology is understood as a branch of biology, ethologists have been particularly concerned with the [[evolution]] of behavior and the understanding of behavior in terms of the [[evolution#theory of natural selection|theory of natural selection]].  In one sense, the first modern ethologist was [[Charles Darwin]], whose book ''The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals'' (1872) has influenced many ethologists. (Darwin’s protégé [[George Romanes]] became one of the founders of comparative psychology, positing a similarity of cognitive processes and mechanisms between animals and humans.)
 +
 
 +
Note, however, that this concept is necessarily speculative. Behaviors are not found as [[fossil]]s and cannot be traced through the geological strata. And concrete evidence for the theory of modification by natural selection is limited to [[microevolution]]—that is, evolution at or below the level of species. The evidence that [[natural selection]] directs changes on the [[macroevolution]]ary level necessarily involves extrapolation from these evidences on the microevolutionary level. Thus, although scientists frequently allude to a particular behavior having evolved by natural selection in response to a particular environment, this involves speculation as opposed to concrete evidence.
  
 
===Animals use fixed action patterns in communication===
 
===Animals use fixed action patterns in communication===
[[Image:Bee dance.png|thumb|left|200px|The honeybee's figure-eight dance is a fixed-action pattern that communicates information to other members of the group: the angle from the sun indicates the direction of a food source; the duration of the dance signifies its distance.]]
+
[[Image:Bee dance.png|thumb|right|250px|The [[honeybee]]'s figure-eight dance is a fixed-action pattern that communicates information to other members of the group: the angle from the sun indicates the direction of a food source; the duration signifies its distance.]]
 +
 
 +
As mentioned above, a ''fixed action pattern (FAP)'' is an [[instinct]]ive [[behavior]]al sequence produced by a [[biological neural network|neural network]] known as the ''innate releasing mechanism'' in response to an external sensory stimulus called the ''sign stimulus'' or ''releaser.'' Once identified by ethologists, FAPs can be compared across species, allowing them to contrast similarities and differences in behavior with similarities and differences in form ([[morphology (biology)|morphology]]).
 +
 
 +
An example of how FAPs work in animal communication is the classic investigation by Austrian ethologist [[Karl von Frisch]] of the so-called "dance language" underlying [[honeybee#communication|bee communication]]. The dance is a mechanism for successful foragers to recruit members of the colony to new sources of [[nectar]] or [[pollen]].
 +
 
 +
===Imprinting is a type of learning behavior===
 +
''Imprinting'' describes any kind of phase-sensitive [[learning]] (i.e., learning that occurs at a particular age or life stage) during which an animal learns the characteristics of some stimulus, which is therefore said to be "imprinted" onto the subject.
  
As mentioned above, a ''fixed action pattern (FAP)'' is an [[instinct]]ive [[behavior]]al sequence that is indivisible and runs to completion.  Fixed action patterns are invariant and are produced by a [[biological neural network|neural network]] known as the ''innate releasing mechanism'' in response to an external [[sensory system|sensory]] stimulus called the ''sign stimulus'' or ''releaser'' (a [[signalling theory|signal]] from one individual to another). Once identified by ethologists, these FAPs could then be compared across species, comparing similarities and differences in behavior to similarities and differences in form ([[morphology (biology)|morphology]]).  
+
The best known form of imprinting is ''filial imprinting,'' in which a young animal learns the characteristics of its parent. [[Konrad Lorenz|Lorenz]] observed that the young of [[waterfowl]] such as [[goose|geese]] spontaneously followed their mothers from almost the first day after they were hatched. Lorenz demonstrated how incubator-hatched geese would imprint on the first suitable moving stimulus they saw within what he called a [[critical period]] of about 36 hours shortly after hatching. Most famously, the goslings would imprint on Lorenz himself (more specifically, on his wading boots).  
  
An example of how FAPs work in animal communication is the classic investigation by Austrian ethologist Karl von Frisch of the so-called "dance language" underlying [[bee learning and communication|bee communication]]. By performing this dance, successful foragers can share with their hive mates information about the direction and distance to patches of flowers yielding nectar or pollen, or both, and to water sources. The dance is a mechanism for recruiting members of the colony to new sites of available resources.
+
''Sexual imprinting,'' which occurs at a later stage of development, is the process by which a young animal learns the characteristics of a desirable mate. For example, male [[zebra finch]]es appear to prefer mates with the appearance of the female bird that rears them, rather than mates of their own type (Immelmann 1972). ''Reverse'' sexual imprinting has also observed: when two individuals live in close domestic proximity during their early years, both are desensitized to later sexual attraction. This phenomenon, known as the ''[[Edvard Westermarck|Westermarck effect]],'' has probably evolved to suppress [[inbreeding]].
  
===Imprinting is a behavior involved in learning===
+
==Relation to comparative psychology==
[[Image:Lorenz.gif|thumb|right|300px|Konrad Z. Lorenz being followed by his imprinted geese]]
+
In order to summarize the defining features of ethology, it might be helpful to compare classical ethology to early work in [[comparative psychology]], an alternative approach to the study of animal behavior that also emerged in the early 20th century. The rivalry between these two fields stemmed in part from disciplinary politics: ethology, which had developed in Europe, failed to gain a strong foothold in [[North America]], where comparative psychology was dominant.
  
''Imprinting'' describes any kind of phase-sensitive [[learning]] (i.e., learning occurring at a particular age or life stage) that is rapid and apparently independent of the consequences of behavior.  It was first used to describe situations in which an animal or person learns the characteristics of some stimulus, which is therefore said to be "imprinted" onto the subject.
+
Broadly speaking, comparative psychology studies general processes, while ethology focuses on adaptive specialization. The two approaches are complementary rather than competitive, but they do lead to different perspectives and sometimes to conflicts of opinion about matters of substance:
  
The best known form of imprinting is ''filial imprinting'', in which a young animal learns the characteristics of its parent.  Lorenz observed that the young of birds such as [[goose|geese]] and [[chicken]]s spontaneously followed their mothers from almost the first day after they were hatched, and he discovered that this response could be imitated by an arbitrary stimulus if the eggs were incubated artificially and the stimulus was presented during a '''critical period''' (a less temporally constrained period is called a '''sensitive period''') that continued for a few days after hatching. Lorenz demonstrated how incubator-hatched geese would imprint on the first suitable moving stimulus they saw within what he called a "[[critical period]]" of about 36 hours shortly after hatching. Most famously, the goslings would imprint on Lorenz himself (more specifically, on his wading boots).  
+
*Comparative psychology construes its study as a branch of [[psychology]] rather than as an outgrowth of [[biology]]. Thus, where comparative psychology sees the study of animal behavior in the context of what is known about human psychology, ethology situates animal behavior in the context of what is known about animal [[anatomy]], [[physiology]], [[neurobiology]], and [[phylogenetic]] history. 
 +
*Comparative psychologists are interested more in similarities than differences in behavior; they are seeking general laws of behavior, especially relating to development, which can then be applied to all animal species, including humans. Hence, early comparative psychologists concentrated on gaining extensive knowledge of the behavior of a few [[species]], while ethologists were more interested in gaining knowledge of behavior in a wide range of species in order to be able to make principled comparisons across [[taxonomy|taxonomic]] groups.  
 +
*Comparative psychologists focused primarily on lab experiments involving a handful of species, mainly [[rat]]s and [[pigeon]]s, whereas ethologists concentrated on behavior in natural situations.  
  
In [[child development]], the term ''filial imprinting'' is used to refer to the process by which a baby learns to recognize its mother and father. The process begins in the womb, when the unborn baby starts to recognize its parents' voices (Kissilevsky et al., 2003). (really?)
+
Since the 1970s, however, animal behavior has become an integrated discipline, with comparative psychologists and ethological animal behaviorists working on similar problems and publishing side by side in the same journals.
  
 
==Recent developments in the field==
 
==Recent developments in the field==
In [[1970]], the [[England|English]] ethologist John H. Crook published an important paper in which he distinguished '''comparative ethology''' from '''social ethology'''. He argued that the ethological studies published to date had focused on the former approachlooking at animals as individualswhereas in the future ethologists would need to concentrate on the social behaviour of animal groups.  
+
In 1970, the [[England|English]] ethologist John H. Crook published an important paper in which he distinguished ''comparative ethology'' from ''social ethology''. He argued that the ethological studies published to date had focused on the former approach—looking at animals as individuals—whereas in the future ethologists would need to concentrate on the social behavior of animal groups.  
  
Since the appearance of [[E. O. Wilson]]'s seminal book ''[[Sociobiology: The New Synthesis]]'' in1975, the study of behaviour has indeed been much more concerned with behavior's social aspects, such as phenotypic altruism and cooperation. Research has also been driven by a more sophisticated version of evolutionary theory associated with Wilson and [[Richard Dawkins]].  
+
Since the appearance of [[E. O. Wilson]]'s seminal book ''Sociobiology: The New Synthesis'' in 1975, ethology has indeed been much more concerned with the social aspects of behavior, such as phenotypic altruism and cooperation. Research has also been driven by a more sophisticated version of evolutionary theory associated with Wilson and [[Richard Dawkins]].  
  
Furthermore, a substantial rapprochement with comparative psychology has occurred, so the modern scientific study of behaviour offers a more or less seamless spectrum of approaches from [[animal cognition]] to more traditional [[comparative psychology]], ethology, [[sociobiology]], behavioural ecology. Evolutionary psychology, an extension of behavioral ecology, looks at commonalities of cognitive processes in humans and other animals as we might expect natural selection to have shaped them. (is evo psych a new term for sociobio?)
+
Furthermore, a substantial rapprochement with comparative psychology has occurred, so the modern scientific study of behavior offers a more or less seamless spectrum of approaches—from animal cognition to comparative psychology, ethology, and behavioral ecology. ''Evolutionary psychology'', an extension of behavioral ecology, looks at commonalities of cognitive processes in humans and other animals as we might expect natural selection to have shaped them. Another promising subfield is ''neuroethology'', concerned with how the structure and functioning of the brain controls behavior and makes [[learning]] possible.
  
==History of the term==
+
==List of influential ethologists==
The term "ethology" is derived from the [[Greek language|Greek]] word "[[ethos]]" (''ήθος''), meaning "[[convention (norm)|custom]]." Other words derived from the Greek word "ethos" include "ethics" and "ethical." The term was first popularized in English by the American [[myrmecologist]] [[William Morton Wheeler]] in 1902.  An earlier, slightly different sense of the term was proposed by [[John Stuart Mill]] in his 1843 <cite>System of Logic</cite>.  He recommended the development of a new science, "ethology," whose purpose would be the explanation of individual and national differences in character, on the basis of [[associationism|associationistic]] [[psychology]].  This use of the word for this purpose was never adopted.
+
The following lis a partial list of scientists who have made notable contributions to the field of ethology (many are comparative psychologists):
 
 
==List of ethologists==
 
People who have made notable contributions to the field of ethology (many are comparative psychologists):
 
  
 
{| width=100%
 
{| width=100%
Line 115: Line 120:
  
 
==References==
 
==References==
*Barnard, C. 2004. ''Animal Behaviour: Mechanism, Development, Function and Evolution''. Harlow, England: Pearson/Prentice Hall. ISBN 0-130-89936-4
+
*Barnard, C. 2004. ''Animal Behaviour: Mechanism, Development, Function and Evolution.'' Harlow, England: Pearson/Prentice Hall. ISBN 0130899364.
*Tinbergen, N. 1991. ''The Study of Instinct''. Reprint ed. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-198-57722-2
+
* Burns, C. 2006. Altruism in nature as manifestation of divine ''energeia.'' ''Zygon'' 41(1): 125-137.
 
+
*Immelmann, K. 1972. Sexual and other long-term aspects of imprinting in birds and other species. ''Advances in the Study of Behavior'' 4:147–74.
==Further reading==
+
* Klein, Z. 2000. [http://www.nel.edu/21_6/NEL21062000X001_Klein_.pdf The ethological approach to the study of human behaviour.] ''Neuroendocrinology Letters'' 21:477-81. Retrieved January 13, 2017.
* Klein, Z. 2000. [http://www.nel.edu/21_6/NEL21062000X001_Klein_.pdf The ethological approach to the study of human behaviour.] ''Neuroendocrinology Letters'' 21:477-81.  
+
*Tinbergen, N. 1991. ''The Study of Instinct.'' Reprint ed. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0198577222.
  
==External links==
 
*[http://www.animal-behaviour.de/ Most popular Internet platform for animal behaviour]
 
*[http://www.kli.ac.at/ Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognitive Research (KLI)]
 
*[http://www.indiana.edu/~animal/ Center for the Integrative Study of Animal behaviour (CISAB)]
 
*[http://cas.bellarmine.edu/tietjen/Ethology/introduction_to_ethology.htm Introduction to ethology]
 
*[http://www.usask.ca/wcvm/herdmed/applied-ethology/ Applied Ethology]
 
*[http://evolution.anthro.univie.ac.at/ishe/ The International Society for Human Ethology] - aims at promoting ethological perspectives in the scientific study of humans worldwide
 
* [http://www-personal.umich.edu/~nesse/fourquestions.pdf The Four Areas of Biology]
 
* [http://homepage.uibk.ac.at/~c720126/humanethologie/ws/medicus/block1/4BQ_E.pdf The Four Areas of Biology AND levels of inquiry]
 
  
  
{{credit|Ethology|136390083}}
+
{{credit|Ethology|136390083|Fixed_action_pattern|138931238|Imprinting_(psychology)|140460744|Neuroethology|113734321|George_Romanes|121936439|Waggle_dance|133603985}}
 
[[Category:Life sciences]]
 
[[Category:Life sciences]]

Latest revision as of 04:36, 22 March 2024

The egg-rolling behavior of the greylag goose is a widely cited example of a fixed-action pattern, one of the key concepts used by ethologists to explain animal behavior.

Ethology is a branch of zoology concerned with the study of animal behavior. Ethologists take a comparative approach, studying behaviors ranging from kinship, cooperation, and parental investment, to conflict, sexual selection, and aggression across a variety of species. Today ethology as a disciplinary label has largely been replaced by behavioral ecology and evolutionary psychology. These rapidly growing fields tend to place greater emphasis on social relationships rather than on the individual animal; however, they retain ethology’s tradition of fieldwork and its grounding in evolutionary theory.

The study of animal behavior touches upon the fact that people receive joy from nature and also typically see themselves in a special role as stewards of creation. Behavior is one aspect of the vast diversity of nature that enhances human enjoyment. People are fascinated with the many behaviors of animals, whether the communication "dance" of honeybees, or the hunting behavior of the big cats, or the altruistic behavior of a dolphin. In addition, humans generally see themselves with the responsibility to love and care for nature.

The study of animal behavior also helps people to understand more about themselves. From an evolutionary point of view, organisms of diverse lineages are related through the process of descent with modification. From a religious point of view, human also stand as “microcosms of nature" (Burns 2006). Thus, the understanding of animals helps to better understand ourselves.

Ethologists engage in hypothesis-driven experimental investigation, often in the field. This combination of lab work with field study reflects an important conceptual underpinning of the discipline: behavior is assumed to be adaptive; in other words, something that makes it better suited in its environment and consequently improves its chances of survival and reproductive success.

Ethology emerged as a discrete discipline in the 1920s, through the efforts of Konrad Lorenz, Karl von Frisch, and Niko Tinbergen, who were jointly awarded the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine for their contributions to the study of behavior. They were in turn influenced by the foundational work of, among others, ornithologists Oskar Heinroth and Julian Huxley and the American myrmecologist (study of ants) William Morton Wheeler, who popularized the term ethology in a seminal 1902 paper.

Important concepts

One of the key ideas of classical ethology is the concept of fixed action patterns (FAPs). FAPs are stereotyped behaviors that occur in a predictable, inflexible sequence in response to an identifiable stimulus from the environment.

Kelp Gull chicks peck at a red spot on their mother's beak to stimulate the regurgitating reflex, another example of a fixed action pattern.

For example, at the sight of a displaced egg near the nest, the greylag goose (Anser anser) will roll the egg back to the others with its beak. If the egg is removed, the animal continues to engage in egg-rolling behavior, pulling its head back as if an imaginary egg is still being maneuvered by the underside of its beak. It will also attempt to move other egg-shaped objects, such as a golf ball, doorknob, or even an egg too large to have been laid by the goose itself (Tinbergen 1991).

Another important concept is filial imprinting, a form of learning that occurs in young animals, usually during a critical, formative period of their lives. During imprinting, a young animal learns to direct some of its social responses to a parent or sibling.

Despite its valuable contributions to the study of animal behavior, classical ethology also spawned problematic general theories that viewed even complex behaviors as genetically hardwired (i.e., innate or instinctive). Models of behavior have since been revised to account for more flexible decision-making processes (Barnard 2003).

Methodology

Tinbergen's four questions for ethologists

The practice of ethological investigation is rooted in hypothesis-driven experimentation. Lorenz's collaborator, Niko Tinbergen, argued that ethologists should consider the following categories when attempting to formulate a hypothesis that explains any instance of behavior:

  • Function: How does the behavior impact the animal's chance of survival and reproduction?
  • Mechanism: What are the stimuli that elicit the response? How has the response been modified by recent learning?
  • Development: How does the behavior change with age? What early experiences are necessary for the behavior to be demonstrated?
  • Evolutionary history: How does the behavior compare with similar behavior in related species? How might the behavior have arisen through the evolutionary development of the species, genus, or group?

The four questions are meant to be complementary, revealing various facets of the motives underlying a given behavior.

Using fieldwork to test hypotheses

As an example of how an ethologist might approach a question about animal behavior, consider the study of hearing in an echolocating bat. A species of bat may use frequency chirps to probe the environment while in flight. A traditional neuroscientific study of the auditory system of the bat would involve anesthetizing it, performing a craniotomy to insert recording electrodes in its brain, and then recording neural responses to pure tone stimuli played from loudspeakers. In contrast, an ideal ethological study would attempt to replicate the natural conditions of the animal as closely as possible. It would involve recording from the animal’s brain while it is awake, producing its natural calls while performing a behavior such as insect capture.

Key principles and concepts

Behaviors are adaptive responses to natural selection

Because ethology is understood as a branch of biology, ethologists have been particularly concerned with the evolution of behavior and the understanding of behavior in terms of the theory of natural selection. In one sense, the first modern ethologist was Charles Darwin, whose book The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (1872) has influenced many ethologists. (Darwin’s protégé George Romanes became one of the founders of comparative psychology, positing a similarity of cognitive processes and mechanisms between animals and humans.)

Note, however, that this concept is necessarily speculative. Behaviors are not found as fossils and cannot be traced through the geological strata. And concrete evidence for the theory of modification by natural selection is limited to microevolution—that is, evolution at or below the level of species. The evidence that natural selection directs changes on the macroevolutionary level necessarily involves extrapolation from these evidences on the microevolutionary level. Thus, although scientists frequently allude to a particular behavior having evolved by natural selection in response to a particular environment, this involves speculation as opposed to concrete evidence.

Animals use fixed action patterns in communication

The honeybee's figure-eight dance is a fixed-action pattern that communicates information to other members of the group: the angle from the sun indicates the direction of a food source; the duration signifies its distance.

As mentioned above, a fixed action pattern (FAP) is an instinctive behavioral sequence produced by a neural network known as the innate releasing mechanism in response to an external sensory stimulus called the sign stimulus or releaser. Once identified by ethologists, FAPs can be compared across species, allowing them to contrast similarities and differences in behavior with similarities and differences in form (morphology).

An example of how FAPs work in animal communication is the classic investigation by Austrian ethologist Karl von Frisch of the so-called "dance language" underlying bee communication. The dance is a mechanism for successful foragers to recruit members of the colony to new sources of nectar or pollen.

Imprinting is a type of learning behavior

Imprinting describes any kind of phase-sensitive learning (i.e., learning that occurs at a particular age or life stage) during which an animal learns the characteristics of some stimulus, which is therefore said to be "imprinted" onto the subject.

The best known form of imprinting is filial imprinting, in which a young animal learns the characteristics of its parent. Lorenz observed that the young of waterfowl such as geese spontaneously followed their mothers from almost the first day after they were hatched. Lorenz demonstrated how incubator-hatched geese would imprint on the first suitable moving stimulus they saw within what he called a critical period of about 36 hours shortly after hatching. Most famously, the goslings would imprint on Lorenz himself (more specifically, on his wading boots).

Sexual imprinting, which occurs at a later stage of development, is the process by which a young animal learns the characteristics of a desirable mate. For example, male zebra finches appear to prefer mates with the appearance of the female bird that rears them, rather than mates of their own type (Immelmann 1972). Reverse sexual imprinting has also observed: when two individuals live in close domestic proximity during their early years, both are desensitized to later sexual attraction. This phenomenon, known as the Westermarck effect, has probably evolved to suppress inbreeding.

Relation to comparative psychology

In order to summarize the defining features of ethology, it might be helpful to compare classical ethology to early work in comparative psychology, an alternative approach to the study of animal behavior that also emerged in the early 20th century. The rivalry between these two fields stemmed in part from disciplinary politics: ethology, which had developed in Europe, failed to gain a strong foothold in North America, where comparative psychology was dominant.

Broadly speaking, comparative psychology studies general processes, while ethology focuses on adaptive specialization. The two approaches are complementary rather than competitive, but they do lead to different perspectives and sometimes to conflicts of opinion about matters of substance:

  • Comparative psychology construes its study as a branch of psychology rather than as an outgrowth of biology. Thus, where comparative psychology sees the study of animal behavior in the context of what is known about human psychology, ethology situates animal behavior in the context of what is known about animal anatomy, physiology, neurobiology, and phylogenetic history.
  • Comparative psychologists are interested more in similarities than differences in behavior; they are seeking general laws of behavior, especially relating to development, which can then be applied to all animal species, including humans. Hence, early comparative psychologists concentrated on gaining extensive knowledge of the behavior of a few species, while ethologists were more interested in gaining knowledge of behavior in a wide range of species in order to be able to make principled comparisons across taxonomic groups.
  • Comparative psychologists focused primarily on lab experiments involving a handful of species, mainly rats and pigeons, whereas ethologists concentrated on behavior in natural situations.

Since the 1970s, however, animal behavior has become an integrated discipline, with comparative psychologists and ethological animal behaviorists working on similar problems and publishing side by side in the same journals.

Recent developments in the field

In 1970, the English ethologist John H. Crook published an important paper in which he distinguished comparative ethology from social ethology. He argued that the ethological studies published to date had focused on the former approach—looking at animals as individuals—whereas in the future ethologists would need to concentrate on the social behavior of animal groups.

Since the appearance of E. O. Wilson's seminal book Sociobiology: The New Synthesis in 1975, ethology has indeed been much more concerned with the social aspects of behavior, such as phenotypic altruism and cooperation. Research has also been driven by a more sophisticated version of evolutionary theory associated with Wilson and Richard Dawkins.

Furthermore, a substantial rapprochement with comparative psychology has occurred, so the modern scientific study of behavior offers a more or less seamless spectrum of approaches—from animal cognition to comparative psychology, ethology, and behavioral ecology. Evolutionary psychology, an extension of behavioral ecology, looks at commonalities of cognitive processes in humans and other animals as we might expect natural selection to have shaped them. Another promising subfield is neuroethology, concerned with how the structure and functioning of the brain controls behavior and makes learning possible.

List of influential ethologists

The following lis a partial list of scientists who have made notable contributions to the field of ethology (many are comparative psychologists):

  • Robert Ardrey
  • George Barlow
  • Patrick Bateson
  • John Bowlby
  • Colleen Cassady St. Clair
  • Raymond Coppinger
  • John H. Crook
  • Marian Stamp Dawkins
  • Richard Dawkins
  • Irenäus Eibl-Eibesfeldt
  • John Fentress
  • Dian Fossey

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Barnard, C. 2004. Animal Behaviour: Mechanism, Development, Function and Evolution. Harlow, England: Pearson/Prentice Hall. ISBN 0130899364.
  • Burns, C. 2006. Altruism in nature as manifestation of divine energeia. Zygon 41(1): 125-137.
  • Immelmann, K. 1972. Sexual and other long-term aspects of imprinting in birds and other species. Advances in the Study of Behavior 4:147–74.
  • Klein, Z. 2000. The ethological approach to the study of human behaviour. Neuroendocrinology Letters 21:477-81. Retrieved January 13, 2017.
  • Tinbergen, N. 1991. The Study of Instinct. Reprint ed. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0198577222.


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.