Difference between revisions of "Same color illusion" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
(Submitted)
 
(10 intermediate revisions by 7 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Claimed}}{{Started}}{{Contracted}}{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}
+
{{Copyedited}}{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}{{Approved}}{{Paid}}
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
+
[[Category:Illusion]]
 
[[Image:Same color illusion.png|thumb|250px|Squares A and B have the same color.]]
 
[[Image:Same color illusion.png|thumb|250px|Squares A and B have the same color.]]
 
[[Image:Same color illusion proof2.png|thumb|right|250px|A rectangle of the same color has been drawn connecting the two squares.]]
 
[[Image:Same color illusion proof2.png|thumb|right|250px|A rectangle of the same color has been drawn connecting the two squares.]]
 
The '''same color illusion''' (also known as '''Adelson's checker shadow illusion''', '''checker shadow illusion''' and '''checker shadow''') is an [[optical illusion]] in which identical shades of gray appear to be different. This illusion is one of many illustrations of the complexity of our visual [[perception]]. The attributes of the physical visual stimulus affect the complex processes in both our [[physiology|physiological]] (of the body) and [[cognition|cognitive]] (of the mind) systems that result in our interpretation, in this case misinterpretation, of the actual figure. Study of this illusion aids in our understanding of the nature of lightness computation in the human visual system, and thus increasing our understanding of how our minds function in informing us about the environment. This type of illusion also is reminds us that human nature has endless [[creativity]] and appreciation for novelty.
 
The '''same color illusion''' (also known as '''Adelson's checker shadow illusion''', '''checker shadow illusion''' and '''checker shadow''') is an [[optical illusion]] in which identical shades of gray appear to be different. This illusion is one of many illustrations of the complexity of our visual [[perception]]. The attributes of the physical visual stimulus affect the complex processes in both our [[physiology|physiological]] (of the body) and [[cognition|cognitive]] (of the mind) systems that result in our interpretation, in this case misinterpretation, of the actual figure. Study of this illusion aids in our understanding of the nature of lightness computation in the human visual system, and thus increasing our understanding of how our minds function in informing us about the environment. This type of illusion also is reminds us that human nature has endless [[creativity]] and appreciation for novelty.
 
+
{{toc}}
 
==Discovery==
 
==Discovery==
The '''same color illusion''' was first published in 1995 by [[Edward H. Adelson]], Professor of [[Vision Science]] at [[Massachusetts Institute of Technology]] (MIT).<ref name="JudgeMaterial"> Edward H. Adelson (2005) [http://web.mit.edu/persci/people/adelson/checkershadow_illusion.html Checkershadow Illusion] Retrieved November 22, 2007.</ref>
+
The same color illusion was first published in 1995 by [[Edward H. Adelson]], Professor of [[Vision Science]] at [[Massachusetts Institute of Technology]] (MIT).<ref name="JudgeMaterial"> Edward H. Adelson, 2005. [http://web.mit.edu/persci/people/adelson/checkershadow_illusion.html Checkershadow Illusion] Retrieved December 4, 2007.</ref>
  
 
==Description==
 
==Description==
 +
The same color illusion shows what appears to be a checkerboard made of dark and light gray squares. Resting in one corner of the checkerboard is a green cylinder that casts a shadow diagonally across the middle of the board. Two of the squares on the board are labeled "A" and "B." When studying the image, square A appears markedly darker than square B, even though they are the exact same shade of gray. If one removes the image surrounding the two squares (or covers it over with a paper), it is clear that they are the same shade.
  
The same color illusion shows what appears to be a checkerboard made of dark and light gray squares. Resting in one corner of the checkerboard is a green cylinder that casts a shadow diagonally across the middle of the board. Two of the squares on the board are labeled "A" and "B". When studying the image, square A appears markedly darker than square B, even though they are the exact same shade of gray. If one removes the image surrounding the two squares (or covers it over with a paper), it is clear that they are the same shade.
+
This illusion is seen on checkerboard patterns in a variety of forms, including what Adelson calls the "corrugated Mondrian." A mondrian is an array of randomly colored, randomly placed rectangles covering a plane surface, and illuminated non-uniformly. In the illusion, it takes the form of a mondrian array drawn to look like a checkerboard painted on a three-dimensional structure, revealing further complexity to our perception.<ref> Edward H. Adelson, 2000. Lightness Perception and Lightness Illusions in Michael S. Gazzaniga, ''The New Cognitive Neurosciences'' (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press). </ref>
 
 
This illusion is seen on checkerboard patterns in a variety of forms, including what Adelson calls the "corrugated Mondrian." A mondrian is an array of randomly colored, randomly placed rectangles covering a plane surface, and illuminated non-uniformly. In the illusion, it takes the form of a mondrian array drawn to look like a checkerboard painted on a three-dimensional structure, revealing further complexity to our perception.<ref> Edward H. Adelson (2000) [http://persci.mit.edu/people/adelson/publications/gazzan.dir/gazzan.htm Lightness Perception and Lightness Illusions] in Michael S. Gazzaniga, ed., ''The New Cognitive Neurosciences''. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Retrieved November 22, 2007. </ref>
 
 
==Explanation==
 
==Explanation==
 
[[Image:Aniopticalillusion.gif|thumb|left|200 px|The two As are the same color.]]
 
[[Image:Aniopticalillusion.gif|thumb|left|200 px|The two As are the same color.]]
Line 23: Line 22:
 
To further complicate matters, the figure is not actually of a checkerboard with alternating light and dark squares in a consistent pattern of alternation in the rows and columns but same color on the diagonal. However, our brain interprets the pattern that way, with the result that although the two gray squares in the same color illusion are actually the same color, their location in the apparent checkerboard implies that one should be dark and the other light to maintain the assumed pattern. The [[Gestalt psychology|Gestalt psychologists]] emphasized the importance of this type of perceptual organization, based on such key concepts as grouping, belongingness, good continuation, proximity, and so on.
 
To further complicate matters, the figure is not actually of a checkerboard with alternating light and dark squares in a consistent pattern of alternation in the rows and columns but same color on the diagonal. However, our brain interprets the pattern that way, with the result that although the two gray squares in the same color illusion are actually the same color, their location in the apparent checkerboard implies that one should be dark and the other light to maintain the assumed pattern. The [[Gestalt psychology|Gestalt psychologists]] emphasized the importance of this type of perceptual organization, based on such key concepts as grouping, belongingness, good continuation, proximity, and so on.
  
Finally, the shadow of the cylinder has soft edges. The visual system tends to ignore gradual changes in light level, focusing instead on sharp edges. Additionally, certain colors such as gray, green, and yellow are more difficult for the visual system to judge accurately. Often, cues for these colors are taken from the surrounding context. As can be seen in the same color illusion, when the context is removed, it is obvious that the two gray squares are the same shade.<ref>Robert Nemiroff and Jerry Bonnell. [http://64.233.169.104/search?q=cache:zfV6C9V3bPoJ:www.bangkokpost.net/education/site2007/nf2jl2407.htm+adelson+checker&hl=en&ct=clnk&cd=9&gl=us&client=firefox-a "The same color illusion"] ''Bangkok Post''. July 24, 2007. Retrieved October 26, 2007.</ref>
+
Finally, the shadow of the cylinder has soft edges. The visual system tends to ignore gradual changes in light level, focusing instead on sharp edges. Additionally, certain colors such as gray, green, and yellow are more difficult for the visual system to judge accurately. Often, cues for these colors are taken from the surrounding context. As can be seen in the same color illusion, when the context is removed, it is obvious that the two gray squares are the same shade.<ref>Nemiroff, Robert and Jerry Bonnell. [http://64.233.169.104/search?q=cache:zfV6C9V3bPoJ:www.bangkokpost.net/education/site2007/nf2jl2407.htm+adelson+checker&hl=en&ct=clnk&cd=9&gl=us&client=firefox-a "The same color illusion"] ''Bangkok Post''. (July 24, 2007). Retrieved December 4, 2007.</ref>
  
 
==Applications==
 
==Applications==
Line 34: Line 33:
  
 
==References ==
 
==References ==
* Gazzaniga, Michael S. (ed.) ''The New Cognitive Neurosciences''. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0262071956
+
* Gazzaniga, Michael S., ed. ''The New Cognitive Neurosciences''. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0262071956
 
* Hoffman, Donald D. 2000. ''Visual Intelligence: How We Create What We See''. W. W. Norton. ISBN 0393319679
 
* Hoffman, Donald D. 2000. ''Visual Intelligence: How We Create What We See''. W. W. Norton. ISBN 0393319679
* Seckel, Al. 2006. ''Optical Illusions: The Science of Visual Perception'' Firefly Books. ISBN 978-1554071722
+
* Seckel, Al. 2006. ''Optical Illusions: The Science of Visual Perception''. Firefly Books.
  
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
All links Retrieved November 16, 2007.
+
All links retrieved December 22, 2022.
* [http://web.mit.edu/persci/people/adelson/checkershadow_description.html Why does the illusion work?]
 
 
* [http://www.archimedes-lab.org/color_optical_illusions.html Colors of the Mind]
 
* [http://www.archimedes-lab.org/color_optical_illusions.html Colors of the Mind]
  
 
{{Credits|Same_color_illusion|156056495|}}
 
{{Credits|Same_color_illusion|156056495|}}

Latest revision as of 01:15, 21 April 2023

Squares A and B have the same color.
A rectangle of the same color has been drawn connecting the two squares.

The same color illusion (also known as Adelson's checker shadow illusion, checker shadow illusion and checker shadow) is an optical illusion in which identical shades of gray appear to be different. This illusion is one of many illustrations of the complexity of our visual perception. The attributes of the physical visual stimulus affect the complex processes in both our physiological (of the body) and cognitive (of the mind) systems that result in our interpretation, in this case misinterpretation, of the actual figure. Study of this illusion aids in our understanding of the nature of lightness computation in the human visual system, and thus increasing our understanding of how our minds function in informing us about the environment. This type of illusion also is reminds us that human nature has endless creativity and appreciation for novelty.

Discovery

The same color illusion was first published in 1995 by Edward H. Adelson, Professor of Vision Science at Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT).[1]

Description

The same color illusion shows what appears to be a checkerboard made of dark and light gray squares. Resting in one corner of the checkerboard is a green cylinder that casts a shadow diagonally across the middle of the board. Two of the squares on the board are labeled "A" and "B." When studying the image, square A appears markedly darker than square B, even though they are the exact same shade of gray. If one removes the image surrounding the two squares (or covers it over with a paper), it is clear that they are the same shade.

This illusion is seen on checkerboard patterns in a variety of forms, including what Adelson calls the "corrugated Mondrian." A mondrian is an array of randomly colored, randomly placed rectangles covering a plane surface, and illuminated non-uniformly. In the illusion, it takes the form of a mondrian array drawn to look like a checkerboard painted on a three-dimensional structure, revealing further complexity to our perception.[2]

Explanation

The two As are the same color.

When looking at the image, the brain and visual system interprets it as a three dimensional image. When interpreting scenes in real life, the visual system has techniques that compensate for different lighting and shadows, enabling us to better interpret objects as they actually exist. There are several specific aspects that contribute to the remarkable success of this illusion.

First, lighter squares surrounded by darker squares appear lighter than average based on local contrast. Conversely, darker squares surrounded by lighter squares appear darker for the same reason. However, the opposite effect is known to occur under certain conditions, as in White's illusion, where it appears as though gray bars surrounded by white stripes are lighter than the gray bars surrounded by black stripes.

To further complicate matters, the figure is not actually of a checkerboard with alternating light and dark squares in a consistent pattern of alternation in the rows and columns but same color on the diagonal. However, our brain interprets the pattern that way, with the result that although the two gray squares in the same color illusion are actually the same color, their location in the apparent checkerboard implies that one should be dark and the other light to maintain the assumed pattern. The Gestalt psychologists emphasized the importance of this type of perceptual organization, based on such key concepts as grouping, belongingness, good continuation, proximity, and so on.

Finally, the shadow of the cylinder has soft edges. The visual system tends to ignore gradual changes in light level, focusing instead on sharp edges. Additionally, certain colors such as gray, green, and yellow are more difficult for the visual system to judge accurately. Often, cues for these colors are taken from the surrounding context. As can be seen in the same color illusion, when the context is removed, it is obvious that the two gray squares are the same shade.[3]

Applications

Illusions of lightness and brightness can help reveal the nature of lightness computation in the human visual system. The same color illusion aptly demonstrates why it is so important to verify what may seem like obvious assumptions when conducting scientific experiments. The human eye and brain can be easily fooled into misinterpretations, which could affect the results of any number of scientific inquiries.

Equally, artists can make use of the sophisticated interpretations that observers make to produce particular lighting effects in their works.

Notes

  1. Edward H. Adelson, 2005. Checkershadow Illusion Retrieved December 4, 2007.
  2. Edward H. Adelson, 2000. Lightness Perception and Lightness Illusions in Michael S. Gazzaniga, The New Cognitive Neurosciences (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
  3. Nemiroff, Robert and Jerry Bonnell. "The same color illusion" Bangkok Post. (July 24, 2007). Retrieved December 4, 2007.

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Gazzaniga, Michael S., ed. The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0262071956
  • Hoffman, Donald D. 2000. Visual Intelligence: How We Create What We See. W. W. Norton. ISBN 0393319679
  • Seckel, Al. 2006. Optical Illusions: The Science of Visual Perception. Firefly Books.

External links

All links retrieved December 22, 2022.

Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.