J. J. Gibson

From New World Encyclopedia

James Jerome Gibson (January 27, 1904–December 11, 1979), was an American psychologist, considered one of the most important twentieth century psychologists in the field of visual perception. In his classic work The Perception of the Visual World (1950) he rejected the fashionable behaviorism for a view based on his own experimental work, which pioneered the idea that animals 'sampled' information from the 'ambient' outside world. He also coined the term 'affordance', meaning the interactive possibilities of a particular object or environment. This concept has been extremely influential in the field of design and ergonomics: see for example the work of Donald Norman who worked with Gibson, and has adapted many of his ideas for his own theories.

Life

James Jerome Gibson was born in McConnelsville, Ohio on January 27, 1904. His father was a railroad man and his mother a teacher, and he grew up on the shores of Lake Michigan. At an early age he was interested in how things appeared in the world. At age eight he noted that when viewed from a train the world "seemed to flow inward when seen from the rear platform and expand outward when seen from the locomotive (Boring and Lindzey 1967, 127). Euclidean geometry intrigued him in high school.

Gibson entered Northwestern University as an undergraduate student, but transferred to Princeton University where he earned both his Bachelors degree (1925) and his Ph.D. in psychology. His doctoral research focused on memory and learning, and constituted the basis of his first publication (Gibson 1928).

His first job was at Smith College, where he taught psychology from 1928 to 1949. There he met Kurt Koffka, the Gestalt psychologist. Gibson never accepted Gestalt psychology, but he did agree with Koffka that the problems of perception were the central problems of psychology (Neisser 1981).

Also at Smith, Gibson met Eleanor Jack, a brilliant psychology student. They married on September 17, 1932. They had two children, James J. and Jean Grier. Eleanor became not only his wife but also his assistant, sharing his views on the how to conduct research.

From 1942, Gibson served in World War II, directing the U.S. Air Force Research Unit in Aviation Psychology. In 1949 he returned to Smith, and began to write his first book, The Perception of the Visual World (1950). By the time it was published, he had moved to Cornell University with a large Air Force grant which supported the research on perceptual learning of both Gibsons. Due to anti-nepotism rules at Cornell, Eleanor was unable to secure a teaching position there, working as a research associate until 1965 when the rules changed. At that time, when Eleanor was appointed to an endowed chair in psychology, the Gibsons became one of the first married couples in the same department at the university.

Gibson continued to teach and carry out research at Cornell for the rest of his life, continuing as professor emeritus after retirement in 1972. He served as division president of the American Psychological Association (APA) and the Eastern Psychological Association. He also received a number of honors, including the Distinguished Scientific Contribution Award in 1961, election to the National Academy of Sciences in 1967, and a Festschrift published in his honor in 1974. Yet it was his research and development of ideas that were of paramount importance to him. He had been a Fulbright scholar at Oxford University, a fellow of the Institute for Advanced Study at Princeton University, and a fellow of the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences at Stanford University. Until the end of his life he continued to write papers and discuss his ideas, holding his "famous Thursday afternoon seminars" attended not only by students but by visiting scholars from around the world (Neisser 1981). He completed his final book in 1979.

J. J. Gibson died in Ithaca on December 11, 1979, at the age of 75. His wife, Eleanor, continued teaching and publishing, including her memoir Perceiving the Affordances: A Portrait of Two Psychologists (2001) that describes the lives, work, and love that she and her husband shared. She died in 2002.

Work

Gibson's greatest desire, according to his own writing, was "to make a contribution to knowledge" (Boring and Lindzey 1967, 141). There is no question that he did just that. His innovative view of perception challenged the traditional psychology of perception based on the philosophical position that external objects causally affect our sense organs which in turn affect the mind, producing an "idea," which may or may not resemble the objects that caused them. Thus, the pioneer of perceptual research, Hermann von Helmholtz, held vision to be a form of unconscious inference: a matter of deriving a probable interpretation for incomplete data. Gibson dismissed this approach:

The conclusions that can be reached from a century of research on perception are insignificant. The knowledge gained from a century of research on sensation is incoherent. We have no adequate theory of perception, and what we have found in this search for sensations is a mixed bag of illusions, physiological curiosities, and bodily feelings. The implications are discouraging. A fresh start has to be made on the problem of perception (Gibson 1992, 229-230).

He made a fresh start, developing an interactionist view of perception and action that focused on information available in the environment, perceived directly through the perceptual system, not constructed by the perceiver (Greeno 1994). Gibson's approach is similar to that of Thomas Reid who realized that sensation was composed of a set of data transfers but declared that these were in some way transparent so that there is a direct connection between perception and the world. This idea is called "direct realism," and Gibson's approach is one of "direct perception."

His work is often divided according to his three books, showing the development of his ideas from his initial The Perception of the Visual World (1950) which first presented the idea of direct perception of our surroundings (the "visual world"), through The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems (1966) which presents invariants in the environment as the origin of perception, to his final The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception (1979) which reflected the development of his thinking and emphasis on meaning through the interaction between perception and action, the "affordances" of the environment.

During this quarter century, he also published many significant articles, several together with his wife Eleanor J. Gibson. In early publications, they argued that perceptual learning is about learning to perceive more of the differentiating qualities of stimuli in the environment, rather than the prevailing view that it was the acquisition of new, more differentiated, responses that are associated with stimuli based on experience (J. J. Gibson and E. J. Gibson 1955a, 1955b). For the Gibsons:

perceptual learning ... consists of responding to variables of physical stimulation not previously responded to. ... learning is always supposed to be a matter of improvement—of getting in closer touch with the environment (Gibson and Gibson 1955a, 34).

Much of Gibson's ideas about perception was developed during his time directing aviation training during World War II. In that context, it was critical that pilots orient themselves based on characteristics of the ground surface observed visually, rather than through data from their vestibular or kinesthetic senses. Gibson discovered invariants in the terrain and sky, the physical world, that were used as the primary perceptual source.

The Senses Considered

Gibson was a brilliant researcher, and his numerous publications revealed a close attention to details in research design as well as innovative theoretical thinking. He realized that the unbounded visual world can be seen directly; it does not have to be inferred through construction (Neisser 1981). Gibson presented his theoretical model for perception in The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems (1966).

Rather than beginning with the sense organs, or with the whole organism (human being or animal) that is the perceiver, he begins with the environment to be perceived. Thus, the questions he asks are not how does the perceiver construct the world from sensory input and past experience, but rather what information is directly available in the environment when a person or animal interacts with it. Gibson suggested that perceptual systems are attuned to the invariants and variables in the environment, and that this information is actively sought through interaction. For Gibson, the environment contains objective information, "invariants" that allow recognition of the properties of surfaces, objects, and so forth.

Critical to Gibson's model is that perception is an active process, involving movement. Thus, for Gibson there is no traditional problem of how to integrate a sequence of retinal images into a coherent object or scene, for perception is perception of change due to continuous movement. The perceptual system seeks invariants in the environment, invariants under changes in illumination, successive sampling of the optic array, transformations due to movement of the observer, or local transformations due to movement or change in objects (Hagen 1992). It is these invariants that allow the observer to perceive the environment, and these invariants are part of the environment and thus perception is not only direct but an accurate view of the world.

However, Gibson did not stop there. He became more and more interested in the question of meaning. The traditional approach, which Gibson naturally rejected, was that objects of perception are in themselves meaningless; meaning is added through higher mental processes such as cognition or memory. Gibson's approach is radically different. Gibson argued that meaning is external to the perceiver and lies in what the environment "affords" the observer.

Affordances

Gibson defined an "affordance" as the quality of an object, or an environment, that allows an individual to perform an action. He originally introduced the term in his 1977 article "The theory of affordances," and explored it more fully in his book The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception:

The affordances of the environment are what it offers the animal, what it provides or furnishes, either for good or ill. The verb to afford is found in the dictionary, but the noun affordance is not. I have made it up. I mean by it something that refers to both the environment and the animal in a way that no existing term does. It implies the complementarity of the animal and the environment (Gibson 1979, 127).

Affordances are "action possibilities" latent in the environment, objectively measurable, and independent of the individual's ability to recognize them, but always in relation to the actor and therefore dependent on their capabilities. "Different layouts afford different behaviors for different animals, and different mechanical encounters" (Gibson 1979, 128). Thus, different objects afford different opportunities for different species—a surface may offer support for a spider but not an elephant—as well as within a species, as what affords sitting for a child may not do so for an adult.

Gibson's theory of affordances has been difficult for many to accept or understand (Greeno 1993)

Legacy

In 1988, Donald Norman appropriated the term "affordances" in the context of human–machine interaction to refer to just those action possibilities which are readily perceivable by an actor. Through his book The Design of Everyday Things (Norman 1988), this interpretation was popularized within the fields of HCI and interaction design. It makes the concept dependent not only on the physical capabilities of the actor, but also their goals, plans, values, beliefs, and past experience. If an actor steps into a room with an armchair and a softball, Gibson's original definition of affordances allows that the actor may throw the recliner and sit on the softball, because that is objectively possible. Norman's definition of (perceived) affordances captures the likelihood that the actor will sit on the recliner and throw the softball. Effectively, Norman's affordances "suggest" how an object may be interacted with. For example, the size and shape of a softball obviously fits nicely in the average human hand, and its density and texture make it perfect for throwing. The user may also bring past experience with similar objects (baseballs, perhaps) to bear when evaluating a new affordance. Norman's 1988 definition makes the concept of affordance relational, rather than subjective or intrinsic. He later explained that this adaptation of the term had been unintended (Norman 1999). However, the definition from his book is much more pertinent to practical design problems from a human-factors approach, which may explain its widespread adoption in the HCI field.

Gibson's work on perception, developed in the context of aviation training, continues to have application for pilots. For example, the training experience should involve realistic environmental settings as much as possible, including in the instructional materials. An unconstrained learning environment is important, since perception is an active process in which the individual seeks information through perceptual systems rather than passively observing what is presented.

Major works

  • Gibson, J. J. 1928. The reproduction of visually perceived forms. Journal of Experimental Psychology 12: 1-39.
  • Gibson, J. J. 1933. Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines. Journal of Experimental Psychology 16: 1-31.
  • Gibson, J. J. 1937. Adaptation with negative after-effect. Psychological Review 44: 222-244.
  • Gibson, J. J., and Eleanor J. Gibson. 1955. Perceptual learning: differentiation or enrichment? Psychological Review 62, 32-41.
  • Gibson, J. J. 1950. The perception of visual surfaces. American Journal of Psychology 63: 367-384.
  • Gibson, J. J. [1950] 1974. The Perception of the Visual World. Greenwood Publishing. ISBN 978-0837178363
  • Gibson, J. J. 1958. Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. British Journal of Psychology 49: 182-194.
  • Gibson, J. J. 1966. The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 0313239614
  • Gibson, J. J. 1972. A theory of Direct Visual Perception. In J. Royce, W. Rozenboom (eds.). The Psychology of Knowing. New York, NY: Gordon & Breach.
  • Gibson, J. J. 1977. The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.). Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology, 67-82. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Gibson, J. J. [1979] 1986. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 0898599598
  • Gibson, J. J. 1982. Reasons for Realism: Selected Essays of James J. Gibson, E. Reed & R. Jones (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Gibson, J. J. 1992. Sensory processes and perception. In A Century of Psychology as a Science, 224-230. S. Koch and D. Leary (eds.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. ISBN 155798171X
  • Gibson, J. J. and E. J. Gibson. 1955a. Perceptual learning: Differentiation or enrichment? Psychological Review 62: 32-41.
  • Gibson, J. J. and E. J. Gibson. 1955b. What is learning in perceptual learning? A reply to Professor Postman. Psychological Review 62: 447-450.
  • Gibson, J. J. and E. J. Gibson. 1957. Continuous perspective transformations and the perception of rigid motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology 54: 129-138.
  • Gibson, E. J., J. J. Gibson, et al. 1959. Motion parallax as a determinant of perceived depth. Journal of Experimental Psychology 58: 40-51.

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Boring, Edwin G., and Gardner Lindzey (eds.). 1967. History of Psychology in Autobiography Volume V. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Gibson, Eleanor J. 2001. Perceiving the Affordances: A Portrait of Two Psychologists. Lawrence Erlbaum. ISBN 978-0805839494
  • Greeno, James G. 1994. Gibson's Affordances Psychological Review 101(2): 336-342. Retrieved July 14, 2008.
  • Haber, Ralph Norman. 1992. Perception: a one-hundred year perspective. In A Century of Psychology as Science, 250-281. S. Koch and D. Leary (eds.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. ISBN 155798171X
  • Hagen, Margaret A. 1992. James J. Gibson's ecological approach to visual perception. In A Century of Psychology as Science, 231-249. S. Koch and D. Leary (eds.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. ISBN 155798171X
  • Hochberg, Julian. 1994. James Jerome Gibson, 1904-1979: Biographical Memoirs. Vol. 63. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
  • Kimble, Gregory A., C. Alan Boneau, and Michael Wertheimer. 1996. Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology Volume II. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. ISBN 978-0805821970
  • MacLeod, Robert Brodie and Herbert L Pick. 1974. Perception: Essays in Honor of James J. Gibson. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. ISBN 978-0801408359
  • Neisser, Ulric. 1981. Obituary: James J. Gibson (1904-1979). American Psychologist, 36(2): 214-215.
  • Norman, Donald A. 1999. Affordance, Conventions and Design Interactions (May 1999): 38-43. Retrieved July 14, 2008.
  • Norman, Donald A. [1988] 2002. The Design of Everyday Things. Basic Books. ISBN 0465067107
  • Reed, Edward S. 1989. James J. Gibson and the Psychology of Perception. Yale University Press. ISBN 978-0300042894

External links

Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.