Difference between revisions of "Dream" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
Line 4: Line 4:
  
 
[[Image:Pierre-Cécile Puvis de Chavannes 003.jpg|thumb|250 px|Pierre-Cécile Puvis de Chavannes: ''The Dream'']]
 
[[Image:Pierre-Cécile Puvis de Chavannes 003.jpg|thumb|250 px|Pierre-Cécile Puvis de Chavannes: ''The Dream'']]
A '''dream''' is the experience of a sequence of images, sounds, ideas, emotions, or other sensations during [[sleep]], especially [[REM sleep]].
+
A '''dream''' is the experience of a sequence of [[image]]s, [[sound]]s, [[idea]]s, [[emotion]]s, or other [[sensation]]s during [[sleep]], especially [[REM sleep]]. The events of dreams are often impossible, or unlikely to occur, in physical reality: they are also outside the control of the dreamer. The exception to this is known as [[lucid dreaming]], in which dreamers realize that they are dreaming, and are sometimes capable of changing their dream [[environment]] and controlling various aspects of the dream. Dreams have long been one of the most puzzling aspects of [[consciousness]] that humankind possesses. Both [[religion]] and [[science]] have tried to define what dreams are, where they come from, and what they mean.  
  
Dreams have long been one of the most puzzling aspects of consciousness that mankind possesses. Both religion and science have tried for years to define what dreams are, where they come from and what they mean, but the process of dreaming and the dreams dreamt remain a mystery. The events of dreams are often impossible, or unlikely to occur, in physical reality: they are also outside the control of the dreamer. The exception to this is known as [[lucid dreaming]], in which dreamers realize that they are dreaming, and are sometimes capable of changing their dream [[environment]] and controlling various aspects of the dream.  
+
People all over the world throughout history have experienced and reported dreams. Some have been, or believed to have been, [[prophet]]ic, messages from the [[spiritual world]] or [[heaven]] giving warnings or announcements of fortune that is to come. For some, dreams are seen as manifestations of our [[unconscious]] [[desire]]s, [[thought]]s, secrets that we repress in our waking lives but which surface as we sleep. For others, a more straightforward and benign explanation is that the [[brain]] processes the experiences of the day into long-term [[memory]] during sleep, sometimes activating a stream of data into consciousness, forming a dream.
  
 
==Dream content==
 
==Dream content==
[[Image:Antonio de Pereda - The Knight's Dream.JPG|thumb|250 px|Antonio de Pereda: ''The Knight's Dream'' (1640)]]
+
[[Image:Antonio de Pereda - The Knight's Dream.JPG|thumb|250 px|left|Antonio de Pereda: ''The Knight's Dream'' (1640)]]
From the 1940s to 1985, [[Calvin S. Hall]] collected more than 50,000 dream reports at [[Western Reserve University]]. In 1966 Hall and Van De Castle published ''The Content Analysis of Dreams'' in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students.<ref name=schneider>Adam Schneider, (2007) [http://psych.ucsc.edu/dreams/Info/content_analysis.html Content Analysis Explained] Retrieved November 3, 2007</ref> It was found that people all over the world dream of mostly the same things. Hall's complete dream reports became publicly available in the mid-1990s by Hall's protégé [[William Domhoff]] allowing further content analysis.
+
[[Calvin S. Hall]] and Van De Castle published ''The Content Analysis of Dreams'' in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students.<ref name=schneider>Adam Schneider, (2007) [http://psych.ucsc.edu/dreams/Info/content_analysis.html Content Analysis Explained] Retrieved November 3, 2007</ref> From the 1940s to 1985, Hall collected more than 50,000 dream reports at [[Western Reserve University]]. Hall's complete dream reports became publicly available in the mid-1990s by Hall's protégé [[William Domhoff]] allowing further content analysis. It was found that people all over the world dream of mostly the same things.  
  
 
===Emotions===
 
===Emotions===
The most common emotion experienced in dreams was [[anxiety]]. Negative emotions are more common than positive feelings.<ref name=schneider/>Some ethnic groups like the [[Yir Yiront]] showed an abnormally high percentage of dreams of an aggressive nature. The U.S. ranks the highest amongst industrialized nations for aggression in dreams with 50 percent of U.S. males reporting aggression in dreams, compared to 32 percent for Dutch men.<ref name=schneider/>
+
The most common emotion experienced in dreams was [[anxiety]]. Negative emotions are more common than positive feelings.<ref name=schneider/> [[Ethnic group]]s showed different percentages of dreams of an [[aggression|aggressive]] nature. The U.S. ranks the highest amongst industrialized nations for aggression in dreams, with 50 percent of U.S. males reporting aggression compared to 32 percent for Dutch men.<ref name=schneider/>
 
 
===Gender differences===
 
In men's dreams 70 percent of the characters are other men, while a female's dreams contain an equal number of men and women.<ref name=schneider/> Men generally had more aggressive feelings in their dreams than women, and children's dreams did not have very much aggression until they reached teen age. These findings parallel much of the current research on gender and gender role comparisons in aggressive behavior. Rather than showing a complementary or compensatory aggressive style, this study supports the view that there is a continuity between our conscious and unconscious styles and personalities.
 
 
 
===Sexual content===
 
Sexual content is not as prevalent in dreams as one might expect. The Hall data analysis shows that sexual dreams show up no more than 10 percent of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid teens.<ref name=schneider/>
 
 
 
===Recurring dreams===
 
 
 
While the content of most dreams is dreamt only once, most people experience recurring dreams—that is, the same dream narrative is experienced over different occasions of sleep. Up to 70 percent of females and 65 percent of males report recurrent dreams.<ref name=schneider/>
 
[[Image:Soldiers-Dream CI AmericanCivilWar.jpg|thumb|250 px|left|''The Soldier's Dream of Home'']]
 
  
 
===Common themes===
 
===Common themes===
Content-analysis studies scientists have identified recurring themes in dreams. Common reported themes have been shown to be: themes relating to school, being chased, sexual experiences, falling, arriving too late, a person now alive being dead, flying, and failing an examination.
+
A number of common themes have been identified in dreams. These include: themes relating to school, being chased, sexual experiences, falling, arriving too late, a person now alive being dead, flying, and failing an examination. In addition, it has been found that 12 percent of people dream only in black and white.
12% of people dream only in black and white.
 
 
<ref>{{cite journal
 
<ref>{{cite journal
 
  | author = Michael Schredl,  Petra Ciric,  Simon Götz,  Lutz Wittmann
 
  | author = Michael Schredl,  Petra Ciric,  Simon Götz,  Lutz Wittmann
Line 41: Line 29:
 
  }}</ref>
 
  }}</ref>
  
==Cultural/Religious Perspectives==
+
===Gender differences===
[[Image:Giotto - Scrovegni - -05- - Joachim's Dream.jpg|thumb|250 px|Giotto di Bondone (1267-1337): ''Legend of Saint Joachim, Joachim's Dream'']]
+
In men's dreams 70 percent of the characters are other men, while women's dreams contain an equal number of men and women.<ref name=schneider/> Men generally had more aggressive feelings in their dreams than women, and children's dreams did not have very much aggression until they reached the teenage years. These findings parallel much of the current research on [[gender]] and [[gender role]] comparisons in aggressive behavior. Rather than showing a complementary or compensatory aggressive style between our thoughts and dreams, these data support the view that there is a continuity between our conscious and unconscious styles and [[personality|personalities]].
Long before science and [[psychology]], religion and cultural beliefs were developed to explain the dream phenomena.  
+
 
Dreams were thought to be part of a spiritual world, and were seen as messages from the [[deity|gods]]. The Abrahamic faiths, [[Christianity]], [[Islam]] and [[Judaism]] believed that there were two sources of dreams: God and [[Satan]].<ref> Islam Online (2007) [http://www.islamonline.net/servlet/Satellite?pagename=IslamOnline-English-Ask_Scholar/FatwaE/FatwaE&cid=1119503546124"Types of Dreams"] Retrieved November 3, 2007</ref>
+
===Sexual content===
Dreams are prolific in the [[Bible]], [[Torah]] and [[Qur'ān]]. Sometimes, these dreams are messages from God, such as when [[Saint Joseph]], the husband of [[Mary, the mother of Jesus|Mary]], when the [[Angel Gabriel]] spoke to him in a dream and told him that the baby Mary was carrying was the [[Jesus|Son of God]]. After the visit of the [[Magi|Three Wise Men]] to them in [[Bethlehem]], an [[angel]] appeared to him and told him to take Mary and Jesus to [[Egypt]] for their safety. The angel appeared again in a dream to tell him when it was safe to return to [[Israel]]. Other times people given more obscure messages that were meant to be interpreted. Both [[Jacob]] and [[Daniel]] are all given the ability to interpret dreams by God. Likewise [[Joseph (Hebrew Bible)|Joseph]] was given the power to interpret dreams and act accordingly, which he did for the [[Pharaoh]] of Egypt, which set in motion the circumstances for the story of [[Exodus]].   
+
Sexual content is not as prevalent in dreams as one might expect. The Hall data analysis shows that sexual dreams show up no more than 10 percent of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid teens.<ref name=schneider/>
[[Image:Michael Lukas Leopold Willmann 001.jpg|thumb|250px|left|Jacob's dream of a ladder of angels]]
+
 
 +
===Recurring dreams===
 +
 
 +
While the content of most dreams is generally experienced in only a single dream, most people experience recurring dreams—that is, the same dream narrative is experienced over different occasions of sleep. Up to 70 percent of females and 65 percent of males report recurrent dreams.<ref name=schneider/>
 +
[[Image:Soldiers-Dream CI AmericanCivilWar.jpg|thumb|250 px|right|''The Soldier's Dream of Home'']]
 +
 
 +
==Cultural and religious perspectives==
 +
[[Image:Giotto - Scrovegni - -05- - Joachim's Dream.jpg|thumb|left|250 px|Giotto di Bondone (1267-1337): ''Legend of Saint Joachim, Joachim's Dream'']]
 +
Long before science and [[psychology]], religion and cultural beliefs were developed to explain the dream phenomena. Dreams were thought to be part of a spiritual world, and were seen as messages from the [[deity|gods]]. The [[Abrahamic faith]]s—[[Christianity]], [[Islam]], and [[Judaism]]—believe that there were two sources of dreams: [[God]] and [[Satan]].<ref> Islam Online (2007) [http://www.islamonline.net/servlet/Satellite?pagename=IslamOnline-English-Ask_Scholar/FatwaE/FatwaE&cid=1119503546124"Types of Dreams"] Retrieved November 3, 2007</ref>
 +
 
 +
Dreams are prolific in the [[Bible]], [[Torah]], and [[Qur'ān]]. Sometimes, these dreams are messages from God, such as of [[Saint Joseph]], the husband of [[Mary, the mother of Jesus|Mary]], when the [[Angel Gabriel]] spoke to him in a dream and told him that the baby Mary was carrying was the [[Jesus|Son of God]]. After the visit of the [[Magi|Three Wise Men]] to them in [[Bethlehem]], an [[angel]] appeared to him and told him to take Mary and Jesus to [[Egypt]] for their safety. The angel appeared again in a dream to tell him when it was safe to return to [[Israel]]. Other times people given more obscure messages that required interpretation. Both [[Jacob]] and [[Daniel]] were all given the ability to interpret dreams by God. Likewise [[Joseph (Hebrew Bible)|Joseph]] was given the power to interpret dreams and act accordingly, which he did for the [[Pharaoh]] of Egypt.   
 +
[[Image:Michael Lukas Leopold Willmann 001.jpg|thumb|250px|right|Jacob's dream of a ladder of angels]]
 +
 
 +
[[Joseph Smith]] (1805 - 1844), founder of the [[Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints]], claimed to have received a visitation from a resurrected [[prophet]] named [[Angel Moroni|Moroni]] that led to his finding and unearthing (in 1827) a long-buried book, inscribed on metal leaves, which contained a record of God's dealings with the ancient Israelite inhabitants of the Americas.
 +
In such cases, the distinction between a dream and a vision of [[spiritual being]]s while awake is blurred. Indeed, people reporting such visitations from beings apparently from the spiritual realm are often unsure as to whether they were awake or dreaming.
 +
 
 
Ancient [[Buddhism]] taught that dreams were the mental projections of a person's desires and fears, an illusory construct developed out of each person's attachment to the illusory world of waking consciousness. [[Tibetan Buddhism]] took the idea one step further, and taught that the dream state was actually one of the ''bardos'', or transition states of conscious that was akin to the spiritual state a person goes through when they die; hence, developing consciousness within this the bardo-dream state would help prepare someone for the ultimate transition in death.<ref> Lama Surya Das. ''Awakening the Buddha Within'' (Broadway Books 1997 ISBN 0767901576)</ref> In India, scholars such as [[Charaka]] (300 B.C.E.) had a similar take on dreams, believing that they were the product of the senses and natural make-up of a person.
 
Ancient [[Buddhism]] taught that dreams were the mental projections of a person's desires and fears, an illusory construct developed out of each person's attachment to the illusory world of waking consciousness. [[Tibetan Buddhism]] took the idea one step further, and taught that the dream state was actually one of the ''bardos'', or transition states of conscious that was akin to the spiritual state a person goes through when they die; hence, developing consciousness within this the bardo-dream state would help prepare someone for the ultimate transition in death.<ref> Lama Surya Das. ''Awakening the Buddha Within'' (Broadway Books 1997 ISBN 0767901576)</ref> In India, scholars such as [[Charaka]] (300 B.C.E.) had a similar take on dreams, believing that they were the product of the senses and natural make-up of a person.
  
[[Shamanism]] in various cultures saw dreams and dream-like states as connections to worlds and realms of the spiritual. Although there were hundreds of different shamanistic traditions, generally, dreams were considered by most to be alternate states of consciousness were as people could visit different spiritual realms, engage in spiritual and physical healing, commune with deities and spirits as well as obtain special knowledge and abilities.<ref> Roger Walsh, ''World of Shamanism: New Views of an Ancient Tradition'' (Llewellyn Publications 2007 ISBN 0738705756)</ref>
+
[[Shamanism]] in various cultures saw dreams and dream-like states as connections to worlds and realms of the spirit. Although there were hundreds of different shamanistic traditions, generally, dreams were considered by most to be alternate states of consciousness where people could visit different spiritual realms, engage in spiritual and physical healing, commune with deities and spirits as well as obtain special knowledge and abilities.<ref> Roger Walsh, ''World of Shamanism: New Views of an Ancient Tradition'' (Llewellyn Publications 2007 ISBN 0738705756)</ref>
  
 
==Psycho-dynamic interpretation of dreams==
 
==Psycho-dynamic interpretation of dreams==
  
Before the rigors of [[biology]] and [[neurology]] were applied to the dream riddle, psychology developed its own theory on what dreams were. Both [[Sigmund Freud]] and [[Carl Jung]] were the first to identify dreams as an interaction between the [[Unconscious mind|unconscious]] and the [[conscious]]. They both asserted that the unconscious is the dominant force of the dream, and in dreams it conveys its own mental activity to the perceptive faculty, but also had many differences with regards to dream analysis.
+
[[Sigmund Freud]] and [[Carl Jung]] were the first to identify dreams as an interaction between the [[Unconscious mind|unconscious]] and the [[conscious]]. They both asserted that the unconscious is the dominant force of the dream, and in dreams it conveys its own mental activity to the perceptive faculty.
  
 
===Freud===
 
===Freud===
[[Sigmund Freud]] arrived at his theory of dreams by research (though he rejected much of the prior work), self-analysis, and psychoanalysis of his patients; as his theory developed, Freud often used dream interpretation to treat his patients, calling dreams "the royal road to a knowledge of the unconscious activities of the mind."  
+
[[Sigmund Freud]] arrived at his theory of dreams by research (though he rejected much of the prior work), self-analysis, and psychoanalysis of his patients. As his theory developed, Freud often used dream interpretation to treat his patients, calling dreams "the royal road to a knowledge of the unconscious activities of the mind."  
 
 
In his book ''[[The Interpretation of Dreams]]'', first published at the end of the nineteenth century, Freud argued that the foundation of all dream content is the fulfillment of wishes, conscious or not. The theory explains that the schism between ''[[superego]]'' and ''[[id]]'' leads to "[[censorship]]" of dreams. The unconscious would "like" to depict the wish fulfilled wholesale, but the preconscious cannot allow it &mdash; the wish (or wishes) within a dream is thus disguised, and, as Freud argues, only an understanding of the structure of the dream-work can explain the dream. In every dream in which he attempts to do so, he is able to establish a multitude of wishes on a variety of levels &mdash; conscious wishes for the immediate future ("I hope I pass this test").
 
  
Freud listed four transformations applied to wishes in order to avoid censorship:
+
In his book ''[[The Interpretation of Dreams]]'', first published at the end of the nineteenth century, Freud argued that the foundation of all dream content is the fulfillment of wishes, conscious or not. The theory explains that the schism between ''[[superego]]'' and ''[[id]]'' leads to "[[censorship]]" of dreams. The unconscious would "like" to depict the wish fulfilled wholesale, but the preconscious cannot allow it &mdash; the wish (or wishes) within a dream is thus disguised, and, as Freud argued, only an understanding of the structure of the dream-work can explain the dream.  Freud listed four transformations applied to wishes in order to avoid censorship:
  
 
* ''Condensation'' &mdash; one dream object stands for several thoughts.
 
* ''Condensation'' &mdash; one dream object stands for several thoughts.
Line 67: Line 68:
 
* ''Symbolism'' &mdash; a symbol replaces an action, person, or idea.
 
* ''Symbolism'' &mdash; a symbol replaces an action, person, or idea.
  
These transformations help to disguise the ''latent'' content, transforming it into the ''manifest'' content, what is actually seen by the dreamer. The basis for all of these systems, he claimed, was "transference," in which a would-be censored wish of the unconscious is given undeserved "psychical energy" (the quantum of attention from consciousness) by attaching to "innocent" thoughts. The basis for these theories was accumulated by Freud through many years of clinical and case study research and summarized in a series of lectures at the University of Vienna during the early twentieth century and replicated in the book ''A general introduction to psychoanalysis" published in 1920.
+
These transformations help to disguise the ''latent'' content, transforming it into the ''manifest'' content, what is actually seen by the dreamer. The basis for all of these systems, he claimed, was "transference," in which a would-be censored wish of the unconscious is given undeserved "psychical energy" (the quantum of attention from consciousness) by attaching to "innocent" thoughts.  
  
Freud further claimed that the counterintuitive nature of [[nightmare]]s represented a clash between the super-ego and the id: the id wishes to see a past wish fulfilled, while the super-ego cannot allow it; he interprets the [[anxiety]] of a nightmare as the super-ego working against the id. (He further claimed that in nearly all cases these anxious dreams are products of [[childhood|infantile]], [[sex]]ual memories.)
+
Freud further claimed that the counterintuitive nature of [[nightmare]]s represented a clash between the super-ego and the id: the id wishes to see a past wish fulfilled, while the super-ego cannot allow it. He interpreted the [[anxiety]] of a nightmare as the super-ego working against the id. (He further claimed that in nearly all cases these anxious dreams are products of [[childhood|infantile]], [[sex]]ual memories.)
  
 
===Jung===
 
===Jung===
  
Dream analysis is central to [[Jungian]] [[analytical psychology]], and forms a critical part of the therapeutic process in classical Jungian analysis. Although not dismissing Freud's model of dream interpretation wholesale, he believed that Freud's notion of dreams as representations of unfulfilled wishes, to be simplistic and naive. Jung was convinced that the scope of dream interpretation was larger, reflecting the richness and complexity of the entire unconscious, both personal and collective. Jung believed the psyche to be a self-regulating organism in which conscious attitudes were likely to be compensated for unconsciously (within the dream) by their opposites.<ref name=storr>Anthony Storr, ''The Essential Jung'' (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press 1999 ISBN 978-0691029351)</ref>  
+
Dream analysis is central to Jungian [[analytical psychology]], and forms a critical part of the therapeutic process in classical Jungian analysis. Although not dismissing Freud's model of dream interpretation wholesale, [[Carl Jung]] believed that Freud's notion of dreams as representations of unfulfilled wishes to be simplistic and naive. Jung was convinced that the scope of dream interpretation was larger, reflecting the richness and complexity of the entire unconscious, both personal and collective. Jung believed the psyche to be a self-regulating organism in which conscious attitudes were likely to be compensated for unconsciously (within the dream) by their opposites.<ref name=storr>Anthony Storr, ''The Essential Jung'' (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press 1999 ISBN 978-0691029351)</ref>  
  
Jung believed that [[archetypes]] such as the [[Anima (Jung)#The female .22animus.22|animus]], the [[Anima (Jung)|anima]], the [[Shadow (psychology)|shadow]] and others manifested themselves in dreams, as dream symbols or figures. Such figures could take the form of an old man, a young maiden or a giant spider as the case may be. Each represents an unconscious attitude that is largely hidden to the conscious mind. Although an integral part of the dreamers psyche, these manifestations were largely autonomous and were perceived by the dreamer to be external personages. Acquaintance with the archetypes as manifested by these symbols serve to increase one's awareness of unconscious attitudes, integrating seemingly disparate parts of the psyche and contributing to the process of holistic self understanding he considered paramount.<ref name=storr/>
+
Jung believed that [[archetype]]s such as the animus, the anima, the shadow, and others manifested themselves in dreams, as dream symbols or figures. Such figures could take the form of an old man, a young maiden, or a giant spider as the case may be. Each represents an unconscious attitude that is largely hidden to the conscious mind. Jung believed that material repressed by the conscious mind, postulated by Freud to comprise the unconscious, was similar to his own concept of the shadow, which in itself is only a small part of the unconscious.
  
Jung believed that material repressed by the conscious mind, postulated by Freud to comprise the unconscious, was similar to his own concept of the shadow, which in itself is only a small part of the unconscious.
+
Although an integral part of the dreamers psyche, these manifestations were largely autonomous and were perceived by the dreamer to be external personages. Acquaintance with the archetypes as manifested by these symbols serves to increase one's awareness of unconscious attitudes, integrating seemingly disparate parts of the psyche and contributing to the process of holistic self understanding he considered paramount.<ref name=storr/>
  
He cautioned against blindly ascribing meaning to dream symbols without a clear understanding of the client's personal situation. Although he acknowledged the universality of archetypal symbols, he contrasted this with the concept of a sign—images having a one to one connotation with their meaning. His approach was to recognise the dynamism and fluidity that existed between symbols and their ascribed meaning. Symbols must be explored for their personal significance to the patient, instead of having the dream conform to some predetermined idea. This prevents dream analysis from devolving into a theoretical and dogmatic exercise that is far removed from the patient's own psychological state. In the service of this idea, he stressed the importance of "sticking to the image"—exploring in depth a client's association with a particular image. This may be contrasted with Freud's free associating which he believed was a deviation, from the salience of the image. He describes for example the image "deal table." One would expect the dreamer to have some associations with this image, and the professed lack of any perceived significance or familiarity whatsoever should make one suspicious. Jung would ask a patient to imagine the image as vividly as possible and to explain it to him as if he had no idea as to what a "deal table" was. Jung stressed the importance of context in dream analysis.
+
Jung cautioned against blindly ascribing meaning to dream symbols without a clear understanding of the client's personal situation. Although he acknowledged the universality of archetypal symbols, he contrasted this with the concept of a sign—images having a one to one connotation with their meaning. His approach was to recognise the dynamism and fluidity that existed between symbols and their ascribed meaning. Symbols must be explored for their personal significance to the patient, instead of having the dream conform to some predetermined idea. Jung stressed the importance of context in dream analysis. In service of this idea, he stressed the importance of "sticking to the image"—exploring in depth a client's association with a particular image. This may be contrasted with Freud's free associating, which Jung believed to be a deviation from the salience of the image.
  
 
===Contemporary dream interpretation===
 
===Contemporary dream interpretation===
In contemporary [[psychoanalysis]], the role of [[dream interpretation]] has been diminished by focusing on other aspects of psychoanalytic views <ref>S. Ringel, (2002). "Dreaming and Listening: A final journey" in ''Clinical Social Work Journal'', 30 (4)</ref> Nevertheless, dreams, and their interpretation, continue to provide a powerful therapeutic focus. Many studies have underlined the importance of dreams in psychoanalysis, and therapeutic work in general. <ref>Pesant, N, Zadra, A. (2004). "Working with dreams in therapy: What do we know and what should we do?" ''Clinical Psychology Review'', 24:489-512.</ref> Further, a growing body of literature supports the ''continuity hypothesis'' of dreams from sleep to waking reality. The continuity hypothesis suggest that the content of dreams is not remote from the waking reality, but, rather, portrays the most prominent feelings, interests and concerns of the individual.<ref>Schredl, M., Landgraf, C., & Zeiler, O. (2003). "Nightmare frequency, nightmare distress and neuroticism" in ''North American Journal of Psychology'', 5, 345–350.</ref>
+
In contemporary [[psychoanalysis]], the role of [[dream interpretation]] has been diminished by focusing on other aspects of psychoanalytic views.<ref>S. Ringel, (2002). "Dreaming and Listening: A final journey" in ''Clinical Social Work Journal'', 30 (4)</ref> Nevertheless, dreams, and their interpretation, continue to provide a powerful therapeutic focus. Many studies have underlined the importance of dreams in psychoanalysis, and therapeutic work in general.<ref>Pesant, N, Zadra, A. (2004). "Working with dreams in therapy: What do we know and what should we do?" ''Clinical Psychology Review'', 24:489-512.</ref> Further, a growing body of literature supports the ''continuity hypothesis'' of dreams from sleep to waking reality. The continuity hypothesis suggest that the content of dreams is not remote from the waking reality, but, rather, portrays the most prominent feelings, interests, and concerns of the individual.<ref>Schredl, M., Landgraf, C., & Zeiler, O. (2003). "Nightmare frequency, nightmare distress and neuroticism" in ''North American Journal of Psychology'', 5, 345–350.</ref>
  
In contrast to  Freud’s idea that the latent content of the dream can be revealed by the implement of the free association, contemporary analysts believe that the unconscious or hidden meaning of the dream is not discovered from the patient’s associations to the dream material. According to them these associations are an additional defense, a disguise, against the patient’s primitive conflicts and reveals only what the dreamer consciously feels or thinks about the dream (Lippman, 2000). Additionally, in modern psychoanalysis dreams are a valuable instrument for examining preverbal conflicts. Disagreeing with Freud’s view that the true meaning of a dream derives from its latent content, contemporary analysts suggest that what one sees in the dream is the dream.
+
==Science of dreams==
 +
During a typical lifespan, a person spends about two hours each night dreaming, a total of about six years dreaming.<ref> Lee Ann Obringer (2006) [http://science.howstuffworks.com/dream4.htm How Dreams Work] Retrieved November 3, 2007 </ref><ref> National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (2007) [http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/brain_basics/understanding_sleep.htm"Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep"] Retrieved November 3, 2007 </ref> Yet, there is no universally agreed-upon biological definition of dreaming. It is unknown where in the brain dreams originate—if there is such a single location—or why dreams occur at all.
  
Modern analysts use the manifest content to understand the patient’s unconscious. They attempt to understand the symbolism of the manifest content of the dream in relation to the total content of the session. During a session in which a patient describes a dream, everything that patient says and does after entering the therapist’s office is considered an association to the dream and is used to untie its manifest content. The representatives of the modern psychoanalytic school are convinced that the patient’s genetic history and unresolved conflicts are revealed in the transference and are symbolized in the patient’s dreams.
+
===REM sleep===
 
+
[[Image:REM.png|thumb|[[Electroencephalography|EEG]] showing brainwaves during REM sleep|thumb|200px]]
==Science of Dreams==
+
General observation shows that dreams are strongly associated with Rapid Eye Movement or [[REM sleep]]. REM sleep is the state of sleep in which [[brain activity]] is most like wakefulness, which is why many researchers believe this is when dreams are strongest, although it could also mean that this is a state from which dreams are most easily remembered.<ref name=laberge> Stephen LaBerge, ''Lucid Dreaming'' (Sounds True 2004 ISBN 1591791502)</ref>  
There is no universally agreed-upon biological definition of dreaming. General observation shows that dreams are strongly associated with [[REM sleep]]. REM sleep is the state of sleep in which [[brain activity]] is most like wakefulness, which is why many researchers believe this is when dreams are strongest, although it could also mean that this is a state from which dreams are most easily remembered.<ref name=laberge> Stephen LaBerge, ''Lucid Dreaming'' (Sounds True 2004 ISBN 1591791502)</ref> During a typical lifespan, a human spends a total of about six years dreaming.<ref> Lee Ann Obringer (2006) [http://science.howstuffworks.com/dream4.htm How Dreams Work] Retrieved November 3, 2007 </ref> (which is about 2 hours each night<ref> National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (2007) [http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/brain_basics/understanding_sleep.htm"Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep"] Retrieved November 3, 2007 </ref> It is unknown where in the brain dreams originate—if there is such a single location—or why dreams occur at all.
 
  
===Discovery of REM===
+
In 1953 [[Eugene Aserinsky]] discovered REM sleep while working in [[Nathaniel Kleitman]]s sleep laboratory. Aserinsky noticed the eyes beneath the subjects' eyelids seemed to be fluttering during periods of their sleep. Kleitman suggested that Aserinsky use a [[polygraph]] machine to record changes in the brain during times when the eye movements occurred. During these sessions, Aserinsky began to notice patterns in the [[brain wave]]s of the volunteers. During one session he awakened a subject who was crying out in his sleep during REM and confirmed an earlier hunch that dreaming was occurring.<ref>William Dement, ''The Sleepwatchers'' (Nychthemeron Press 1996 ISBN 0964933802)</ref> In 1953 Kleitman and Aserinsky published the ground-breaking study in [[Science (journal)|''Science'']].<ref name="as-science">Eugene Aserinsky and Nathaniel Kleitman (1953) [http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/reprint/118/3062/273.pdf Regularly Occurring Periods of Eye Motility, and Concomitant Phenomena, During Sleep] ''Science'' Volume 4, 273-274, September 1953. Retrieved November 17, 2007.</ref>
[[Image:REM.png|thumb|[[Electroencephalography|EEG]] showing brainwaves during REM sleep|thumb|200px]]
 
In 1953 [[Eugene Aserinsky]] discovered [[Rapid eye movement|REM sleep]] while working in the lab of his Doctor of Philosophy advisor [[Nathaniel Kleitman]]. While observing sleepers for Kleitman's sleep lab, Aserinsky noticed the eyes beneath the subjects' eyelids seemed to be fluttering. He proposed studying these eye movements to Kleitman, who agreed, suggesting that Aserinsky use a [[polygraph]] machine to record changes in the brain. During these sessions observing sleepers, Aserinsky began to notice patterns in the [[brain waves]] of the volunteers. During one session he awakened a subject who was crying out in his sleep during REM and confirmed an earlier hunch that dreaming was occurring. <ref>William Dement, ''The Sleepwatchers'' (Nychthemeron Press 1996 ISBN 0964933802)</ref> In 1953 Kleitman and Aserinsky published the ground-breaking study in [[Science (journal)|''Science'']].<ref name="as-science">{{cite journal
 
| author = Aserinsky and Kleitman
 
| date =
 
| year = 1953
 
| month = September
 
| title =
 
| journal = Science
 
| volume = 4
 
| issue =
 
| pages = 273-274
 
| doi = 10.1126/science.118.3062.273
 
| id =
 
| url = http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/reprint/118/3062/273.pdf
 
| language =
 
| format =
 
| accessdate =
 
}}</ref>
 
  
In 1976 [[J. Allan Hobson]] and Robert McCarly proposed a new theory that changed dream research, challenging the previously held [[Freudian]] view of dreams as subconscious wishes to be interpreted. The [[activation synthesis theory]] asserts that the sensory experiences are fabricated by the cortex as a means of interpreting [[chaos theory|chaotic]] signals from the [[pons]]. They propose that in REM sleep, the ascending [[cholinergic]] [[PGO]] (ponto-geniculo-occipital) waves stimulate higher [[midbrain]] and [[forebrain]] cortical structures, producing rapid eye movements. The activated forebrain then synthesizes the dream out of this internally generated information.<ref> Evie Bentley, ''Awareness: Biorhythms, Sleep and Dreaming'' (Routledge 1999 ISBN 0415188733)</ref> They assume that the same structures that induce REM sleep also generate sensory information.
+
In 1976, [[J. Allan Hobson]] and Robert McCarly proposed the [[activation synthesis theory]] of dreams. This theory asserts that the sensory experiences are fabricated by the [[cortex]] as a means of interpreting [[chaos theory|chaotic]] signals from the [[pons]]. They propose that in REM sleep, the ascending [[cholinergic]] [[PGO]] (ponto-geniculo-occipital) waves stimulate higher [[midbrain]] and [[forebrain]] cortical structures, producing rapid eye movements. The activated forebrain then synthesizes the dream out of this internally generated information.<ref> Evie Bentley, ''Awareness: Biorhythms, Sleep and Dreaming'' (Routledge 1999 ISBN 0415188733)</ref> They assumed that the same structures that induce REM sleep also generate sensory information.
  
 
===Role of forebrain===
 
===Role of forebrain===
Hobson's 1976 research suggested that the signals interpreted as dreams originated in the brain stem during REM sleep. However, research by Mark Solms suggests that dreams are generated in the [[forebrain]], and that REM sleep and dreaming are not directly related.<ref>{{cite book
+
Hobson's 1976 research suggested that the signals interpreted as dreams originated in the brain stem during REM sleep. However, research by Mark Solms suggested that dreams are generated in the [[forebrain]], and that REM sleep and dreaming are not directly related.<ref>{{cite book
 
| last = Solms
 
| last = Solms
 
| first = M.
 
| first = M.
Line 122: Line 105:
 
| edition = 23(6)
 
| edition = 23(6)
 
| pages = 793-1121
 
| pages = 793-1121
}}</ref> While working in the neurosurgery department at hospitals in [[Johannesburg]] and [[London]], Solms had access to patients with various brain injuries. He began to question patients about their dreams and confirmed that patients with damage to the [[parietal lobe]] stopped dreaming; this finding was in line with Hobson's 1977 theory. However, Solms did not encounter cases of loss of dreaming with patients having brain stem damage. This observation forced him to question Hobson's prevailing theory which marked the brain stem as the source of the signals interpreted as dreams. Solms viewed the idea of dreaming as a function of many complex brain structures as validating Freudian dream theory, an idea that drew criticism from Hobson.<ref>Andrea Rock, ''The Mind at Night: The New Science of How and Why we Dream'' (Basic Books 2004 ISBN 0465070698)</ref>
+
}}</ref> While working in the neurosurgery department at hospitals in [[Johannesburg]] and [[London]], Solms had access to patients with various brain injuries. He began to question patients about their dreams and confirmed that patients with damage to the [[parietal lobe]] stopped dreaming; this finding was in line with Hobson's theory. However, Solms did not encounter cases of loss of dreaming with patients having brain stem damage, which went against Hobson's notion of the brain stem as the source of the signals interpreted as dreams. Solms viewed the idea of dreaming as a function of many complex brain structures as validating Freudian dream theory, an idea that drew criticism from Hobson.<ref>Andrea Rock, ''The Mind at Night: The New Science of How and Why we Dream'' (Basic Books 2004 ISBN 0465070698)</ref>
  
 
===Continual-activation theory===
 
===Continual-activation theory===
Combining Hobson's activation synthesis hypothesis with Solms's findings, the continual-activation theory of dreaming presented by Jie Zhang proposes that dreaming is a result of brain activation and synthesis; at the same time, dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. Zhang hypothesizes that the function of sleep is to process, encode and transfer the data from the temporary memory to the long-term memory, though there is not much evidence backing up this so-called "consolidation." NREM sleep processes the conscious-related memory (declarative memory), and REM sleep processes the unconscious related memory (procedural memory).  
+
Combining Hobson's activation synthesis hypothesis with Solms's findings, [[Jie Zhang]] proposed the continual-activation theory of dreaming, that dreaming is a result of brain activation and synthesis while, at the same time, dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. Zhang hypothesized that the function of sleep is to process, encode, and transfer the data from the temporary [[memory]] to the long-term memory. In this model, NREM sleep processes the conscious-related memory (declarative memory), and REM sleep processes the unconscious related memory (procedural memory).  
  
Zhang assumes that during REM sleep, the unconscious part of a brain is busy processing the procedural memory; meanwhile, the level of activation in the conscious part of the brain will descend to a very low level as the inputs from the sensory are basically disconnected. This will trigger the "continual-activation" mechanism to generate a data stream from the memory stores to flow through the conscious part of the brain. Zhang suggests that this pulse-like brain activation is the inducer of each dream. He proposes that, with the involvement of the brain associative thinking system, dreaming is, thereafter, self-maintained with the dreamer's own thinking until the next pulse of memory insertion. This explains why dreams have both characteristics of continuity (within a dream) and sudden changes (between two dreams).<ref>{{cite book
+
Zhang assumed that during REM sleep, the unconscious part of a brain is busy processing the procedural memory; meanwhile, the level of activation in the conscious part of the brain will descend to a very low level as the inputs from the sensory are basically disconnected. This will trigger the "continual-activation" mechanism to generate a data stream from the memory stores to flow through the conscious part of the brain. Zhang suggested that this pulse-like brain activation is the inducer of each dream. He proposed that, with the involvement of the brain associative thinking system, dreaming is, thereafter, self-maintained with the dreamer's own thinking until the next pulse of memory insertion. This explains why dreams have both characteristics of continuity (within a dream) and sudden changes (between two dreams).<ref>{{cite book
 
| last = Zhang
 
| last = Zhang
 
| first = Jie
 
| first = Jie
Line 145: Line 128:
 
}}</ref>
 
}}</ref>
  
 
===Hippocampus and memory===
 
 
A 2001 study showed evidence that illogical locations, characters, and dream flow may help the brain strengthen the linking and consolidation of [[semantic memory|semantic memories]]. These conditions may occur because, during REM sleep, the flow of information between the hippocampus and neocortex is reduced.<ref>{{cite journal  
 
A 2001 study showed evidence that illogical locations, characters, and dream flow may help the brain strengthen the linking and consolidation of [[semantic memory|semantic memories]]. These conditions may occur because, during REM sleep, the flow of information between the hippocampus and neocortex is reduced.<ref>{{cite journal  
 
  | author = R. Stickgold, J. A. Hobson, R. Fosse, M. Fosse1  
 
  | author = R. Stickgold, J. A. Hobson, R. Fosse, M. Fosse1  
Line 158: Line 139:
 
  | pages = 1052 - 1057
 
  | pages = 1052 - 1057
 
  | doi = 10.1126
 
  | doi = 10.1126
  }}</ref> Increasing levels of the [[stress (medicine)|stress]] hormone [[Cortisol]] late in sleep (often during REM sleep) cause this decreased communication. One stage of [[memory consolidation]] is the linking of distant but related memories. Payne and Nadal hypothesize that these memories are then consolidated into a smooth narrative, similar to a process that happens when memories are created under stress.<ref>{{cite journal
+
  }}</ref> Increasing levels of the [[stress (medicine)|stress]] hormone [[cortisol]] late in sleep (often during REM sleep) cause this decreased communication. One stage of [[memory consolidation]] is the linking of distant but related memories. Payne and Nadal hypothesized that these memories are then consolidated into a smooth narrative, the dream, similar to a process that happens when memories are created under stress.<ref>{{cite journal
 
  | author = Jessica D. Payne and Lynn Nadel1
 
  | author = Jessica D. Payne and Lynn Nadel1
 
  | year = 2004
 
  | year = 2004
Line 168: Line 149:
 
  }}</ref>
 
  }}</ref>
  
 +
==Other associated phenomena==
 +
===Lucid dreaming===
  
 +
Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one's state while dreaming. The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified. Many people, including scientists and psychologists have started to acknowledge the benefits of lucid dreaming. If developed as a skill, a person who is able to achieve lucid dreaming can often explore the complexities of their sub-conscious, helping to deal with past trauma, fears, anxieties, and can promote mental health.<ref name=laberge/>
  
==Other associated phenomena==
+
===Recalling dreams===
===Lucid dreaming===
+
Many people have difficulty recalling their dreams. Researchers refer to these types of dreams as "no content dream reports."<ref name=koulack>David Koulack, ''To Catch A Dream'' (SUNY Press 1991 ISBN 0791405028)</ref> It appears that such dreams are characterized by relatively little [[affect (psychology)|affect]]. Factors such as salience, arousal, and interference play a role in dream recall and dream recall failure.<ref name=koulack>
  
Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one's state while dreaming. The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified. Many people, including scientists and psychologists have started to acknowledge the benefits of lucid dreaming. If developed as a skill, a person who is able to achieve lucid dreaming can often explore the complexities of their sub-conscious, helping to deal with past trauma, fears, anxieties and can promote mental health.<ref name=laberge/>
+
Keeping a dream journal appears to be a useful technique to improve dream recall. It is quite common to not remember much of a dream on first waking, but by lying still, not letting concerns of the day occupy the mind, with sufficient concentration the entire dream may be recalled.<ref name=laberge/>  
  
 
===Dreams of absent-minded transgression===
 
===Dreams of absent-minded transgression===
Dreams of absent-minded transgression (DAMT) are dreams wherein the dreamer absentmindedly performs an action that he or she has been trying to stop (one classic example is of a quitting smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette). Subjects who have had DAMT have reported awaking with intense feelings of guilt. Some studies have shown that DAMT are positively related with successfully stopping the behavior, when compared to control subjects who did not experience these dreams.<ref>{{cite web
+
Dreams of absent-minded transgression (DAMT) are dreams wherein the dreamer absentmindedly performs an action that he or she has been trying to stop (one classic example is of a former smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette). Subjects who have had DAMT have reported awaking with intense feelings of guilt. Some studies have shown that DAMT are positively related with successfully stopping the behavior, when compared to control subjects who did not experience these dreams.<ref>{{cite web
 
| url = http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=1757662&dopt=Abstract
 
| url = http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=1757662&dopt=Abstract
 
| title = Dream of absent-minded transgression: an empirical study of a cognitive withdrawal symptom
 
| title = Dream of absent-minded transgression: an empirical study of a cognitive withdrawal symptom
Line 187: Line 171:
  
 
===Dreaming as a skeptical argument===
 
===Dreaming as a skeptical argument===
{{main|dream argument}}
 
While one dreams a non-lucid dream, one will not realize one is dreaming (one classic example is a child dreaming that they are using the toilet and end up wetting the bed because they don't realize that they are in a dream). This has led philosophers to the idea that one could be dreaming right now (or at least one cannot be certain that one is not dreaming). First formally introduced by [[Zhuangzi]] and popularized by Hindu beliefs, the dream argument has become one of the most popular [[skeptical hypothesis|skeptical hypotheses]]. Out of the major religions and philosophies in the world, [[Buddhism]] makes most use of this argument.
 
  
===Recalling dreams===
+
While dreaming a non-lucid dream, the dreamer does not realize that they are dreaming. The classic example is a child dreaming that they are using the toilet who wets the bed because they do not realize that they are in a dream. This lack of awareness has led philosophers to the idea that one could be dreaming right now (or at least one cannot be certain that one is not dreaming). First formally introduced by [[Zhuangzi]] and popularized by [[Hinduism|Hindu]] beliefs, the dream argument has become one of the most popular hypotheses in support of [[skepticism]]. It was formally introduced to western philosophy by [[Descartes]] in the seventeenth century in his ''Meditations on First Philosophy''.
Many humans have difficulty recalling their dreams. Researchers refer to these types of dreams as "no content dream reports."<ref name=koulack>David Koulack, ''To Catch A Dream'' (SUNY Press 1991 ISBN 0791405028)</ref> It is thought that such dreams are characterized by relatively little [[affect (psychology)|affect]]. Factors such as salience, arousal and interference play a role in dream recall and dream recall failure.<ref name=koulack> Many authorities on the issue believe that a useful technique to improve dream recall is to keep a [[dream journal]]. [[Stephen LaBerge]], author of ''Exploring the World of Lucid Dreaming'', also suggests that one must lie perfectly still upon awaking from a dream, not letting concerns of the day occupy the mind. It is quite common to not remember much of what has just been dreamed, but LaBerge maintains that with sufficient concentration, the entire dream may be recalled.<ref name=laberge/> Another sufficient method to recall a dream is to wake at least 5 minutes after dreaming.
 
  
 
===Déjà vu===
 
===Déjà vu===
 
{{main|Déjà vu}}
 
{{main|Déjà vu}}
The theory of déjà vu dealing with dreams indicates that the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something could be attributed to having dreamt about a similar situation or place, and forgetting about it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or place while awake.
+
The theory of [[déjà vu]] dealing with dreams indicates that the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something could be attributed to having dreamed about a similar situation or place, and forgetting about it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or place while awake.
  
 
===Dream incorporation===
 
===Dream incorporation===
In one use of the term, "dream incorporation" is a phenomenon whereby an external stimulus, usually an auditory one, becomes a part of a dream, eventually then awakening the dreamer. There is a famous painting by [[Salvador Dalí]] that depicts this concept, titled ''Dream Caused by the Flight of a Bee around a Pomegranate a Second Before Awakening'' (1944).
+
In one use of the term, "dream incorporation" is a phenomenon whereby an external stimulus, usually an auditory one, becomes a part of a dream, eventually then awakening the dreamer. There is a famous [[painting]] by [[Salvador Dalí]] that depicts this concept, entitled ''Dream Caused by the Flight of a Bee around a Pomegranate a Second Before Awakening'' (1944).
  
The term "dream incorporation" is also used in research examining the degree to which preceding daytime events become elements of dreams. Recent studies suggest that events in the day immediately preceding, and those about a week before, have the most influence <ref>
+
The term "dream incorporation" is also used in research examining the degree to which preceding daytime events become elements of dreams. Studies suggest that events in the day immediately preceding, and those about a week before, have the most influence.<ref>
[http://www.asdreams.org/2003/abstracts/genevieve_alain.htm"Abstract"] (2003) 20th Annual International Conference of the Association for the Study of Dreams Retrieved November 3, 2007</ref>.
+
[http://www.asdreams.org/2003/abstracts/genevieve_alain.htm"Abstract"] (2003) 20th Annual International Conference of the Association for the Study of Dreams Retrieved November 3, 2007</ref>
  
 
==Notes==
 
==Notes==

Revision as of 00:51, 17 November 2007


Pierre-Cécile Puvis de Chavannes: The Dream

A dream is the experience of a sequence of images, sounds, ideas, emotions, or other sensations during sleep, especially REM sleep. The events of dreams are often impossible, or unlikely to occur, in physical reality: they are also outside the control of the dreamer. The exception to this is known as lucid dreaming, in which dreamers realize that they are dreaming, and are sometimes capable of changing their dream environment and controlling various aspects of the dream. Dreams have long been one of the most puzzling aspects of consciousness that humankind possesses. Both religion and science have tried to define what dreams are, where they come from, and what they mean.

People all over the world throughout history have experienced and reported dreams. Some have been, or believed to have been, prophetic, messages from the spiritual world or heaven giving warnings or announcements of fortune that is to come. For some, dreams are seen as manifestations of our unconscious desires, thoughts, secrets that we repress in our waking lives but which surface as we sleep. For others, a more straightforward and benign explanation is that the brain processes the experiences of the day into long-term memory during sleep, sometimes activating a stream of data into consciousness, forming a dream.

Dream content

Antonio de Pereda: The Knight's Dream (1640)

Calvin S. Hall and Van De Castle published The Content Analysis of Dreams in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students.[1] From the 1940s to 1985, Hall collected more than 50,000 dream reports at Western Reserve University. Hall's complete dream reports became publicly available in the mid-1990s by Hall's protégé William Domhoff allowing further content analysis. It was found that people all over the world dream of mostly the same things.

Emotions

The most common emotion experienced in dreams was anxiety. Negative emotions are more common than positive feelings.[1] Ethnic groups showed different percentages of dreams of an aggressive nature. The U.S. ranks the highest amongst industrialized nations for aggression in dreams, with 50 percent of U.S. males reporting aggression compared to 32 percent for Dutch men.[1]

Common themes

A number of common themes have been identified in dreams. These include: themes relating to school, being chased, sexual experiences, falling, arriving too late, a person now alive being dead, flying, and failing an examination. In addition, it has been found that 12 percent of people dream only in black and white. [2]

Gender differences

In men's dreams 70 percent of the characters are other men, while women's dreams contain an equal number of men and women.[1] Men generally had more aggressive feelings in their dreams than women, and children's dreams did not have very much aggression until they reached the teenage years. These findings parallel much of the current research on gender and gender role comparisons in aggressive behavior. Rather than showing a complementary or compensatory aggressive style between our thoughts and dreams, these data support the view that there is a continuity between our conscious and unconscious styles and personalities.

Sexual content

Sexual content is not as prevalent in dreams as one might expect. The Hall data analysis shows that sexual dreams show up no more than 10 percent of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid teens.[1]

Recurring dreams

While the content of most dreams is generally experienced in only a single dream, most people experience recurring dreams—that is, the same dream narrative is experienced over different occasions of sleep. Up to 70 percent of females and 65 percent of males report recurrent dreams.[1]

The Soldier's Dream of Home

Cultural and religious perspectives

Giotto di Bondone (1267-1337): Legend of Saint Joachim, Joachim's Dream

Long before science and psychology, religion and cultural beliefs were developed to explain the dream phenomena. Dreams were thought to be part of a spiritual world, and were seen as messages from the gods. The Abrahamic faiths—Christianity, Islam, and Judaism—believe that there were two sources of dreams: God and Satan.[3]

Dreams are prolific in the Bible, Torah, and Qur'ān. Sometimes, these dreams are messages from God, such as of Saint Joseph, the husband of Mary, when the Angel Gabriel spoke to him in a dream and told him that the baby Mary was carrying was the Son of God. After the visit of the Three Wise Men to them in Bethlehem, an angel appeared to him and told him to take Mary and Jesus to Egypt for their safety. The angel appeared again in a dream to tell him when it was safe to return to Israel. Other times people given more obscure messages that required interpretation. Both Jacob and Daniel were all given the ability to interpret dreams by God. Likewise Joseph was given the power to interpret dreams and act accordingly, which he did for the Pharaoh of Egypt.

Jacob's dream of a ladder of angels

Joseph Smith (1805 - 1844), founder of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints, claimed to have received a visitation from a resurrected prophet named Moroni that led to his finding and unearthing (in 1827) a long-buried book, inscribed on metal leaves, which contained a record of God's dealings with the ancient Israelite inhabitants of the Americas. In such cases, the distinction between a dream and a vision of spiritual beings while awake is blurred. Indeed, people reporting such visitations from beings apparently from the spiritual realm are often unsure as to whether they were awake or dreaming.

Ancient Buddhism taught that dreams were the mental projections of a person's desires and fears, an illusory construct developed out of each person's attachment to the illusory world of waking consciousness. Tibetan Buddhism took the idea one step further, and taught that the dream state was actually one of the bardos, or transition states of conscious that was akin to the spiritual state a person goes through when they die; hence, developing consciousness within this the bardo-dream state would help prepare someone for the ultimate transition in death.[4] In India, scholars such as Charaka (300 B.C.E.) had a similar take on dreams, believing that they were the product of the senses and natural make-up of a person.

Shamanism in various cultures saw dreams and dream-like states as connections to worlds and realms of the spirit. Although there were hundreds of different shamanistic traditions, generally, dreams were considered by most to be alternate states of consciousness where people could visit different spiritual realms, engage in spiritual and physical healing, commune with deities and spirits as well as obtain special knowledge and abilities.[5]

Psycho-dynamic interpretation of dreams

Sigmund Freud and Carl Jung were the first to identify dreams as an interaction between the unconscious and the conscious. They both asserted that the unconscious is the dominant force of the dream, and in dreams it conveys its own mental activity to the perceptive faculty.

Freud

Sigmund Freud arrived at his theory of dreams by research (though he rejected much of the prior work), self-analysis, and psychoanalysis of his patients. As his theory developed, Freud often used dream interpretation to treat his patients, calling dreams "the royal road to a knowledge of the unconscious activities of the mind."

In his book The Interpretation of Dreams, first published at the end of the nineteenth century, Freud argued that the foundation of all dream content is the fulfillment of wishes, conscious or not. The theory explains that the schism between superego and id leads to "censorship" of dreams. The unconscious would "like" to depict the wish fulfilled wholesale, but the preconscious cannot allow it — the wish (or wishes) within a dream is thus disguised, and, as Freud argued, only an understanding of the structure of the dream-work can explain the dream. Freud listed four transformations applied to wishes in order to avoid censorship:

  • Condensation — one dream object stands for several thoughts.
  • Displacement — a dream object's psychical importance is assigned to an object that does not raise the censor's suspicions.
  • Representation — a thought is translated to visual images.
  • Symbolism — a symbol replaces an action, person, or idea.

These transformations help to disguise the latent content, transforming it into the manifest content, what is actually seen by the dreamer. The basis for all of these systems, he claimed, was "transference," in which a would-be censored wish of the unconscious is given undeserved "psychical energy" (the quantum of attention from consciousness) by attaching to "innocent" thoughts.

Freud further claimed that the counterintuitive nature of nightmares represented a clash between the super-ego and the id: the id wishes to see a past wish fulfilled, while the super-ego cannot allow it. He interpreted the anxiety of a nightmare as the super-ego working against the id. (He further claimed that in nearly all cases these anxious dreams are products of infantile, sexual memories.)

Jung

Dream analysis is central to Jungian analytical psychology, and forms a critical part of the therapeutic process in classical Jungian analysis. Although not dismissing Freud's model of dream interpretation wholesale, Carl Jung believed that Freud's notion of dreams as representations of unfulfilled wishes to be simplistic and naive. Jung was convinced that the scope of dream interpretation was larger, reflecting the richness and complexity of the entire unconscious, both personal and collective. Jung believed the psyche to be a self-regulating organism in which conscious attitudes were likely to be compensated for unconsciously (within the dream) by their opposites.[6]

Jung believed that archetypes such as the animus, the anima, the shadow, and others manifested themselves in dreams, as dream symbols or figures. Such figures could take the form of an old man, a young maiden, or a giant spider as the case may be. Each represents an unconscious attitude that is largely hidden to the conscious mind. Jung believed that material repressed by the conscious mind, postulated by Freud to comprise the unconscious, was similar to his own concept of the shadow, which in itself is only a small part of the unconscious.

Although an integral part of the dreamers psyche, these manifestations were largely autonomous and were perceived by the dreamer to be external personages. Acquaintance with the archetypes as manifested by these symbols serves to increase one's awareness of unconscious attitudes, integrating seemingly disparate parts of the psyche and contributing to the process of holistic self understanding he considered paramount.[6]

Jung cautioned against blindly ascribing meaning to dream symbols without a clear understanding of the client's personal situation. Although he acknowledged the universality of archetypal symbols, he contrasted this with the concept of a sign—images having a one to one connotation with their meaning. His approach was to recognise the dynamism and fluidity that existed between symbols and their ascribed meaning. Symbols must be explored for their personal significance to the patient, instead of having the dream conform to some predetermined idea. Jung stressed the importance of context in dream analysis. In service of this idea, he stressed the importance of "sticking to the image"—exploring in depth a client's association with a particular image. This may be contrasted with Freud's free associating, which Jung believed to be a deviation from the salience of the image.

Contemporary dream interpretation

In contemporary psychoanalysis, the role of dream interpretation has been diminished by focusing on other aspects of psychoanalytic views.[7] Nevertheless, dreams, and their interpretation, continue to provide a powerful therapeutic focus. Many studies have underlined the importance of dreams in psychoanalysis, and therapeutic work in general.[8] Further, a growing body of literature supports the continuity hypothesis of dreams from sleep to waking reality. The continuity hypothesis suggest that the content of dreams is not remote from the waking reality, but, rather, portrays the most prominent feelings, interests, and concerns of the individual.[9]

Science of dreams

During a typical lifespan, a person spends about two hours each night dreaming, a total of about six years dreaming.[10][11] Yet, there is no universally agreed-upon biological definition of dreaming. It is unknown where in the brain dreams originate—if there is such a single location—or why dreams occur at all.

REM sleep

thumb

General observation shows that dreams are strongly associated with Rapid Eye Movement or REM sleep. REM sleep is the state of sleep in which brain activity is most like wakefulness, which is why many researchers believe this is when dreams are strongest, although it could also mean that this is a state from which dreams are most easily remembered.[12]

In 1953 Eugene Aserinsky discovered REM sleep while working in Nathaniel Kleitmans sleep laboratory. Aserinsky noticed the eyes beneath the subjects' eyelids seemed to be fluttering during periods of their sleep. Kleitman suggested that Aserinsky use a polygraph machine to record changes in the brain during times when the eye movements occurred. During these sessions, Aserinsky began to notice patterns in the brain waves of the volunteers. During one session he awakened a subject who was crying out in his sleep during REM and confirmed an earlier hunch that dreaming was occurring.[13] In 1953 Kleitman and Aserinsky published the ground-breaking study in Science.[14]

In 1976, J. Allan Hobson and Robert McCarly proposed the activation synthesis theory of dreams. This theory asserts that the sensory experiences are fabricated by the cortex as a means of interpreting chaotic signals from the pons. They propose that in REM sleep, the ascending cholinergic PGO (ponto-geniculo-occipital) waves stimulate higher midbrain and forebrain cortical structures, producing rapid eye movements. The activated forebrain then synthesizes the dream out of this internally generated information.[15] They assumed that the same structures that induce REM sleep also generate sensory information.

Role of forebrain

Hobson's 1976 research suggested that the signals interpreted as dreams originated in the brain stem during REM sleep. However, research by Mark Solms suggested that dreams are generated in the forebrain, and that REM sleep and dreaming are not directly related.[16] While working in the neurosurgery department at hospitals in Johannesburg and London, Solms had access to patients with various brain injuries. He began to question patients about their dreams and confirmed that patients with damage to the parietal lobe stopped dreaming; this finding was in line with Hobson's theory. However, Solms did not encounter cases of loss of dreaming with patients having brain stem damage, which went against Hobson's notion of the brain stem as the source of the signals interpreted as dreams. Solms viewed the idea of dreaming as a function of many complex brain structures as validating Freudian dream theory, an idea that drew criticism from Hobson.[17]

Continual-activation theory

Combining Hobson's activation synthesis hypothesis with Solms's findings, Jie Zhang proposed the continual-activation theory of dreaming, that dreaming is a result of brain activation and synthesis while, at the same time, dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. Zhang hypothesized that the function of sleep is to process, encode, and transfer the data from the temporary memory to the long-term memory. In this model, NREM sleep processes the conscious-related memory (declarative memory), and REM sleep processes the unconscious related memory (procedural memory).

Zhang assumed that during REM sleep, the unconscious part of a brain is busy processing the procedural memory; meanwhile, the level of activation in the conscious part of the brain will descend to a very low level as the inputs from the sensory are basically disconnected. This will trigger the "continual-activation" mechanism to generate a data stream from the memory stores to flow through the conscious part of the brain. Zhang suggested that this pulse-like brain activation is the inducer of each dream. He proposed that, with the involvement of the brain associative thinking system, dreaming is, thereafter, self-maintained with the dreamer's own thinking until the next pulse of memory insertion. This explains why dreams have both characteristics of continuity (within a dream) and sudden changes (between two dreams).[18][19]

A 2001 study showed evidence that illogical locations, characters, and dream flow may help the brain strengthen the linking and consolidation of semantic memories. These conditions may occur because, during REM sleep, the flow of information between the hippocampus and neocortex is reduced.[20] Increasing levels of the stress hormone cortisol late in sleep (often during REM sleep) cause this decreased communication. One stage of memory consolidation is the linking of distant but related memories. Payne and Nadal hypothesized that these memories are then consolidated into a smooth narrative, the dream, similar to a process that happens when memories are created under stress.[21]

Other associated phenomena

Lucid dreaming

Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one's state while dreaming. The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified. Many people, including scientists and psychologists have started to acknowledge the benefits of lucid dreaming. If developed as a skill, a person who is able to achieve lucid dreaming can often explore the complexities of their sub-conscious, helping to deal with past trauma, fears, anxieties, and can promote mental health.[12]

Recalling dreams

Many people have difficulty recalling their dreams. Researchers refer to these types of dreams as "no content dream reports."[22] It appears that such dreams are characterized by relatively little affect. Factors such as salience, arousal, and interference play a role in dream recall and dream recall failure.Cite error: Closing </ref> missing for <ref> tag

Dreaming as a skeptical argument

While dreaming a non-lucid dream, the dreamer does not realize that they are dreaming. The classic example is a child dreaming that they are using the toilet who wets the bed because they do not realize that they are in a dream. This lack of awareness has led philosophers to the idea that one could be dreaming right now (or at least one cannot be certain that one is not dreaming). First formally introduced by Zhuangzi and popularized by Hindu beliefs, the dream argument has become one of the most popular hypotheses in support of skepticism. It was formally introduced to western philosophy by Descartes in the seventeenth century in his Meditations on First Philosophy.

Déjà vu

Main article: Déjà vu

The theory of déjà vu dealing with dreams indicates that the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something could be attributed to having dreamed about a similar situation or place, and forgetting about it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or place while awake.

Dream incorporation

In one use of the term, "dream incorporation" is a phenomenon whereby an external stimulus, usually an auditory one, becomes a part of a dream, eventually then awakening the dreamer. There is a famous painting by Salvador Dalí that depicts this concept, entitled Dream Caused by the Flight of a Bee around a Pomegranate a Second Before Awakening (1944).

The term "dream incorporation" is also used in research examining the degree to which preceding daytime events become elements of dreams. Studies suggest that events in the day immediately preceding, and those about a week before, have the most influence.[23]

Notes

  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 1.5 Adam Schneider, (2007) Content Analysis Explained Retrieved November 3, 2007
  2. Michael Schredl, Petra Ciric, Simon Götz, Lutz Wittmann (November, 2004). Typical Dreams: Stability and Gender Differences. The Journal of Psychology 138 (6): 485.
  3. Islam Online (2007) "Types of Dreams" Retrieved November 3, 2007
  4. Lama Surya Das. Awakening the Buddha Within (Broadway Books 1997 ISBN 0767901576)
  5. Roger Walsh, World of Shamanism: New Views of an Ancient Tradition (Llewellyn Publications 2007 ISBN 0738705756)
  6. 6.0 6.1 Anthony Storr, The Essential Jung (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press 1999 ISBN 978-0691029351)
  7. S. Ringel, (2002). "Dreaming and Listening: A final journey" in Clinical Social Work Journal, 30 (4)
  8. Pesant, N, Zadra, A. (2004). "Working with dreams in therapy: What do we know and what should we do?" Clinical Psychology Review, 24:489-512.
  9. Schredl, M., Landgraf, C., & Zeiler, O. (2003). "Nightmare frequency, nightmare distress and neuroticism" in North American Journal of Psychology, 5, 345–350.
  10. Lee Ann Obringer (2006) How Dreams Work Retrieved November 3, 2007
  11. National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (2007) "Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep" Retrieved November 3, 2007
  12. 12.0 12.1 Stephen LaBerge, Lucid Dreaming (Sounds True 2004 ISBN 1591791502)
  13. William Dement, The Sleepwatchers (Nychthemeron Press 1996 ISBN 0964933802)
  14. Eugene Aserinsky and Nathaniel Kleitman (1953) Regularly Occurring Periods of Eye Motility, and Concomitant Phenomena, During Sleep Science Volume 4, 273-274, September 1953. Retrieved November 17, 2007.
  15. Evie Bentley, Awareness: Biorhythms, Sleep and Dreaming (Routledge 1999 ISBN 0415188733)
  16. Solms, M. (2000). Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms, 23(6), Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 793-1121. 
  17. Andrea Rock, The Mind at Night: The New Science of How and Why we Dream (Basic Books 2004 ISBN 0465070698)
  18. Zhang, Jie (2004). Memory process and the function of sleep, 6-6, Journal of Theoretics. Retrieved 2006-03-13. 
  19. Zhang, Jie (2005). Continual-activation theory of dreaming, Dynamical Psychology. Retrieved 2006-03-13. 
  20. R. Stickgold, J. A. Hobson, R. Fosse, M. Fosse1 (November 2001). Sleep, Learning, and Dreams: Off-line Memory Reprocessing. Science 294 (5544): 1052 - 1057.
  21. Jessica D. Payne and Lynn Nadel1 (2004). Sleep, dreams, and memory consolidation: The role of the stress hormone cortisol. LEARNING & MEMORY: 671-678.
  22. David Koulack, To Catch A Dream (SUNY Press 1991 ISBN 0791405028)
  23. "Abstract" (2003) 20th Annual International Conference of the Association for the Study of Dreams Retrieved November 3, 2007

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Crick, F. & Mitchinson, G. (1983) "The function of dream sleep." Nature 304, pp. 111-114.
  • Tarnow, E. (2003) "How Dreams And Memory May Be Related." Neuro-Psychoanalysis 5(2), pp. 177-182.
  • Van de Castle, Robert L. (1994). Our Dreaming Mind. New York: Ballantine Books. ISBN 0345396669. 
  • Artemidorus, The Oneirocritica of Artemidorus, University Microfilms, New Haven (1971).
  • Sigmund Freud, The Interpretation of Dreams, Avon, (1980).
  • Carl Jung, Dreams, Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey (1974).
  • Bernard Dieterle, Manfred Engel (ed.): The Dream and the Enlightenment / Le Rêve et les Lumières. Paris: Honoré Champion 2003; ISBN 2-7453-0672-3.
  • Clara E. Hill, Working with Dreams in Psychotherapy (1996) ISBN 1-57230-092-2
  • Koulack, David "To Catch A Dream: Explorations of Dreaming," SUNY Press, New York (1991).
  • Jayne Gackenbach, Stephen LaBerge, Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain: Perspectives on Lucid Dreaming, Plenum Publishing Corporation, New York (1988).
  • Acharya, Pt. Shriram Sharma, Sleep, Dreams and Spiritual Reflections (2000)
  • Patricia L Garfield, Creative Dreaming (1974) ISBN 0-671-21903-0
  • Will Phillips, Every Dreamer's Handbook: A Step-by-Step Guide to Understanding and Benefiting From Your Dreams, ISBN 1-57566-048-2, Totonada Press (1994)
  • Namkhai Norbu Rinpoche, Dream Yoga and the Practice of Natural Light, Snow Lion Publications, Ithaca, New York (1992).
  • Tenzin Wangyal Rinpoche, The Tibetan Yogas of Dream and Sleep, Snow Lion Publications, Ithaca, New York (1998).
  • Carlos Castaneda, The Art of Dreaming, Harper Collins (1993)
  • Freud, Sigmund, A general introduction to psychoanalysis, Boni & Liveright, NY, 1920.
  • James A. Hall, Jungian Dream Interpretation: A Handbook of Theory and Practice, Inner City Books, 1983, ISBN 0-919123-12-0
  • Ellman, S. (2000). Dreams: commentary on paper by Hazel Ipp. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 10(1), 143–157.
  • Lippman, P. (1996). On dreams and interpersonal psychoanalysis. Psychoanal.Dialogues, 6:831–846.
  • Lippman, P. (2000). Nocturnes: on listening to dreams. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press, Inc.
  • Fosshage, J. (2000). The organizing functions of dreaming—a contemporary psychoanalytic model: Commentary on paper by Hazel Ipp. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 10(1), 103–117.

External links

All links Retrieved November 16, 2007.

Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.