Difference between revisions of "Sign language" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
(Claimed)
(fixed)
Line 3: Line 3:
 
[[Category:Linguistics]]
 
[[Category:Linguistics]]
 
{{Claimed}}
 
{{Claimed}}
[[Image:Sign language interpreter - CSD 2006 Cologne 5.jpg|thumb|right|Sign language interpreter on stage]]
 
  
A '''sign language''' (also '''signed language''') is a [[language]] which uses [[manual communication]] instead of [[sound]] to convey meaning - simultaneously combining handshapes, orientation and movement of the [[hands]], [[arms]] or [[body]], and [[facial expressions]] to express fluidly a speaker's [[thoughts]]. Sign languages commonly develop in [[deaf community|deaf communities]], which can include interpreters and friends and families of deaf people as well as people who are [[deaf]] or [[hard of hearing]] themselves. They are also used by people with speech impairments such as [[Aphasia]].
 
  
When people using different signed languages meet, communication is significantly easier than when people of different spoken languages meet. Sign language, in this respect, gives access to an international deaf community.
+
[[Image:Interp.jpg|thumb|right|Two sign language [[Intepreters]] working as a team for a school.]]
  
However, contrary to popular belief, sign language is not universal.   Wherever communities of deaf people exist, sign languages develop, but as with spoken languages, these vary from region to region. They are not based on the spoken language in the country of origin; in fact their complex spatial grammars are markedly different. However, various signed "modes" of spoken languages have been developed, such as [[Signed English]] and [[Warlpiri Sign Language]].  Hundreds of sign languages are in use around the world and are at the core of local [[Deaf culture]]s. Some sign languages have obtained some form of [[Recognition of sign languages|legal recognition]], while others have no status at all.
+
A '''sign language''' (also '''signed language''') is a [[language]] which uses [[manual communication]], [[body language]] and lip patterns instead of [[sound]] to convey meaning—simultaneously combining hand shapes, orientation and movement of the [[hand]]s, [[arm]]s or [[body]], and [[facial expressions]] to express fluidly a speaker's [[thoughts]]. Sign languages commonly develop in [[deaf community|deaf communities]], which can include interpreters and friends and families of deaf people as well as people who are [[deaf]] or [[hard of hearing]] themselves.
 +
 
 +
As is the case in spoken language, sign language differs from one region to another. However, when people using different signed languages meet, communication is significantly easier than when people of different spoken languages meet.{{Fact|date=July 2007}}  Sign language, in this respect, gives access to an international deaf community.  Sign language is however not universal, and many different sign languages exist that are mostly mutually unintelligible.
 +
 
 +
Wherever communities of deaf people exist, sign languages develop, in fact their complex spatial grammars are markedly different than spoken language. In many cases, various signed "modes" of spoken languages have been developed, such as [[Signed English]] and [[Warlpiri Sign Language]].  Hundreds of sign languages are in use around the world and are at the core of local [[Deaf culture]]s. Some sign languages have obtained some form of [[Recognition of sign languages|legal recognition]], while others have no status at all.
 +
 
 +
Exemplary for the mature status of sign languages is the growing body of sign language poetry, and other stage performances. The [http://jeroenarendsen.nl/2007/01/sign-language-poetry-and-theatre-from-draadloos/ poetic mechanisms] available to signing poets are not all available to a speaking poet. This offers new, exciting ways for poems to reach and move the audience.  
  
 
==History of sign language==
 
==History of sign language==
{{main|History of sign language}}
 
The recorded history of sign language in Western society extends from the 16th century. In 1755, [[Abbé de l'Épée]] founded the first public school for deaf children in Paris; Laurent Clerc was arguably its most famous graduate. He went to the United States with Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet to found the [[American School for the Deaf]].  It evolved into [[Gallaudet University]], the only liberal arts university for the deaf in the world.
 
  
==Geographic distribution of sign languages==
+
The recorded history of sign language in Western society extends from the 16th century. In 1755, [[Abbé de l'Épée]] founded the first public school for deaf children in Paris; Laurent Clerc was arguably its most famous graduate. He went to the United States with [[Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet]] to found the [[American School for the Deaf]] in Hartford, Connecticut.<ref>{{cite web | last=Canlas | first=Loida | title=Laurent Clerc: Apostle to the Deaf People of the New World | publisher=[[Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Center]] at [[Gallaudet University]] | date=2006-07-10 | url=http://clerccenter.gallaudet.edu/Literacy/MSSDLRC/clerc/ | accessdate=2007-01-06 }}</ref> Gallaudet's son, [[Edward Miner Gallaudet]] founded the first college for the deaf in 1857, which in 1864 became [[Gallaudet University]], the only liberal arts university for the deaf in the world. Gallaudet University is still present today; it is located in Washington DC.
 +
 
 
Generally, each spoken language has a sign language counterpart in as much as each linguistic population will contain Deaf members who will generate a sign language. In much the same way that geographical or cultural forces will isolate populations and lead to the generation of different and distinct spoken languages, the same forces operate on signed languages and so they tend to maintain their identities through time in roughly the same areas of influence as the local spoken languages. This occurs even though sign languages have no relation to the spoken languages of the lands in which they arise. There are notable exceptions to this pattern, however, as some geographic regions sharing a spoken language have multiple, unrelated signed languages.
 
Generally, each spoken language has a sign language counterpart in as much as each linguistic population will contain Deaf members who will generate a sign language. In much the same way that geographical or cultural forces will isolate populations and lead to the generation of different and distinct spoken languages, the same forces operate on signed languages and so they tend to maintain their identities through time in roughly the same areas of influence as the local spoken languages. This occurs even though sign languages have no relation to the spoken languages of the lands in which they arise. There are notable exceptions to this pattern, however, as some geographic regions sharing a spoken language have multiple, unrelated signed languages.
  
Line 22: Line 25:
 
[[International Sign]], formerly known as Gestuno, is used mainly at international Deaf events such as the [[Deaflympics]] and meetings of the [[World Federation of the Deaf]]. Recent studies claim that while International Sign is a kind of a [[pidgin]], they conclude that it is more complex than a typical pidgin and indeed is more like a full signed language.
 
[[International Sign]], formerly known as Gestuno, is used mainly at international Deaf events such as the [[Deaflympics]] and meetings of the [[World Federation of the Deaf]]. Recent studies claim that while International Sign is a kind of a [[pidgin]], they conclude that it is more complex than a typical pidgin and indeed is more like a full signed language.
  
==Use of signs in hearing communities==
+
The recorded '''history of sign language''' in Western society extends from the 16th century, as a [[visual language]] or method of communication. Sign language is a system of conventional gestures, mimic, hand signs and finger spelling, the use of hand positions to represent the letters of the alphabet. Signs usually represent complete ideas, not only words. Sign language is mainly used by [[deaf]] and [[speech disorder|mute]] people in order to communicate.
  
[[Gesture]] is a typical component of spoken languages. More elaborate systems of [[manual communication]] have developed in situations where speech is not practical or permitted, such as [[monastery|cloistered religious communities]], [[scuba diving]], [[Television studio|television recording studios]], loud workplaces, [[stock exchange]]s, in [[baseball]], while hunting (by groups such as the Kalahari [[bushmen]]), or in the game [[Charades]].  In [[Rugby Union]] the Referee uses a limited but defined set of signs to communicate his/her decisions to the spectators. Recently, there has been a movement to teach and encourage the use of sign language with toddlers before they learn to talk and with non-deaf or hard-of-hearing children with other causes of speech impairment or delay.  This is typically referred to as [[Baby Sign]].
+
=== Development of Sign Language ===
  
[[Image:Jw_signs_convention.jpg|thumbnail|300px|left|[[Jehovah's Witnesses]] Convention in ''[[Chilean Sign Language]]''.]]
+
Standardized sign languages have been used in Italy since the [[17th century]] and in France since the [[18th century]] for the instruction of the deaf. [[Old French Sign Language]] was developed and used in [[Paris]] by the [[Abbé de l'Épée|l’Abbé Charles Michel de l’Epée]] in his school for the deaf. These languages were always modeled after the natural sign languages already in use by the deaf cultures in their area of origin, often with additions to show aspects of the grammar of the local spoken languages.
  
On occasion, where the prevalence of deaf people is high enough, a deaf sign language has been taken up by an entire local community. Famous examples of this include [[Martha's Vineyard Sign Language]] in the [[USA]], [[Kata Kolok]] in a village in [[Bali]], [[Adamorobe Sign Language]] in [[Ghana]] and [[Yucatec Maya sign language]] in [[Mexico]]. In such communities deaf people are not socially disadvantaged.
+
In 1755, Abbé de l'Épée founded the first public school for deaf children in Paris. His lessons were based upon his observations of deaf people signing with hands in the streets of Paris. Synthesized with French grammar, it evolved into the French Sign Language. [[Laurent Clerc]], a graduate and former teacher of the French School, went to the [[United States]] with [[Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet]] to found an American School for the deaf.  
  
Many [[Australian Aboriginal sign languages]] arose in a context of extensive speech taboos, such as during mourning and initiation rites. They are or were especially highly developed among the [[Warlpiri Sign Language|Warlpiri]], [[Warumungu]], [[Dieri]], [[Kaytetye]], [[Arrernte]], [[Warlmanpa]], and are based on their respective spoken languages.
+
The 18th permanent school for the deaf was established in Hartford, Connecticut; others followed. In 1817, Clerc and Gallaudet founded the American Asylum for the Deaf and Dumb (now the American School for the Deaf). In 1864, a college for deaf people was founded in [[Washington D.C.]] Its charter was signed by [[Abraham Lincoln]] and was named “Gallaudet College.
  
A [[pidgin]] sign language arose among tribes of [[Native Americans in the United States|American Indians]] in the [[Great Plains]] region of [[North America]] (see [[Plains Indians]]). It was used to communicate among tribes with different spoken [[language]]s. There are especially users today among the [[Crow tribe|Crow]], [[Cheyenne]], and [[Arapaho]].  Unlike other sign languages developed by hearing people, it shares the spatial grammar of deaf sign languages.
 
  
 
==Linguistics of sign==
 
==Linguistics of sign==
In linguistic terms, sign languages are as rich and complex as any oral language, despite the common misconception that they are not "real languages". Professional [[linguistics|linguists]] have studied many sign languages and found them to have every linguistic component required to be classed as true languages.  
+
In linguistic terms, sign languages are as rich and complex as any oral language, despite the common misconception that they are not "real languages." Professional [[linguistics|linguists]] have studied many sign languages and found them to have every linguistic component required to be classed as true languages.  
  
Sign languages are not [[pantomime]] - in other words, signs are largely arbitrary and have no necessary visual relationship to their referent, much as most spoken language is not [[onomatopoeic]]. Nor are they a visual rendition of an oral language. They have rich, complex [[grammar]]s of their own, and can be used to discuss any topic, from the simple and concrete to the lofty and abstract.
+
Sign languages are not [[pantomime]] - in other words, signs are largely arbitrary and have no necessary visual relationship to their referent, much as most spoken language is not [[onomatopoeic]]. Nor are they a visual rendition of an oral language. They have complex [[grammar]]s of their own, and can be used to discuss any topic, from the simple and concrete to the lofty and abstract.
  
Sign languages, like oral languages, organize elementary, meaningless units ([[phoneme]]s; once called [[chereme]]s in the case of sign languages) into meaningful [[semantic]] units. The elements of a sign are '''H'''andshape (or Handform), '''O'''rientation (or Palm Orientation), '''L'''ocation (or Place of Articulation), '''M'''ovement, and Non-manual markers (or Facial '''E'''xpression), summarised in the [[acronym]] HOLME.
+
Sign languages, like oral languages, organize elementary, meaningless units ([[phoneme]]s; once called [[chereme]]s in the case of sign languages) into meaningful [[semantic]] units. The elements of a sign are '''H'''andshape (or Handform), '''O'''rientation (or Palm Orientation), '''L'''ocation (or Place of Articulation), '''M'''ovement, and Non-manual markers (or Facial '''E'''xpression), summarised in the [[acronym]] '''HOLME'''.
  
 
Common linguistic features of deaf sign languages are extensive use of [[Classifier (linguistics)|classifiers]], a high degree of [[inflection]], and a [[Topic-comment language|topic-comment]] [[syntax]]. Many unique linguistic features emerge from sign languages' ability to produce meaning in different parts of the visual field simultaneously. For example, the recipient of a signed message can read meanings carried by the hands, the facial expression and the body posture in the same moment. This is in contrast to oral languages, where the sounds that comprise words are mostly sequential (tone being an exception).
 
Common linguistic features of deaf sign languages are extensive use of [[Classifier (linguistics)|classifiers]], a high degree of [[inflection]], and a [[Topic-comment language|topic-comment]] [[syntax]]. Many unique linguistic features emerge from sign languages' ability to produce meaning in different parts of the visual field simultaneously. For example, the recipient of a signed message can read meanings carried by the hands, the facial expression and the body posture in the same moment. This is in contrast to oral languages, where the sounds that comprise words are mostly sequential (tone being an exception).
Line 51: Line 53:
  
 
Similarly, countries which use a single oral language throughout may have two or more sign languages; whereas an area that contains more than one oral language might use only one sign language.
 
Similarly, countries which use a single oral language throughout may have two or more sign languages; whereas an area that contains more than one oral language might use only one sign language.
 +
 +
==Use of signs in hearing communities==
 +
[[Gesture]] is a typical component of spoken languages. More elaborate systems of [[manual communication]] have developed in situations where speech is not practical or permitted, such as [[monastery|cloistered religious communities]], [[scuba diving]], [[Television studio|television recording studios]], loud workplaces, [[stock exchange]]s, in [[baseball]], while hunting (by groups such as the Kalahari [[bushmen]]), or in the game [[Charades]].  In [[Rugby Union]] the Referee uses a limited but defined set of signs to communicate his/her decisions to the spectators. Recently, there has been a movement to teach and encourage the use of sign language with toddlers before they learn to talk and with non-deaf or hard-of-hearing children with other causes of speech impairment or delay.  This is typically referred to as [[Baby Sign]].
 +
 +
[[Image:Jw signs convention.jpg|thumbnail|300px|left|[[Jehovah's Witnesses]] Convention in ''[[Chilean Sign Language]]''.]]
 +
 +
On occasion, where the prevalence of deaf people is high enough, a deaf sign language has been taken up by an entire local community. Famous examples of this include [[Martha's Vineyard Sign Language]] in the [[USA]], [[Kata Kolok]] in a village in [[Bali]], [[Adamorobe Sign Language]] in [[Ghana]] and [[Yucatec Maya sign language]] in [[Mexico]]. In such communities deaf people are not socially disadvantaged.
 +
 +
Many [[Australian Aboriginal sign languages]] arose in a context of extensive speech taboos, such as during mourning and initiation rites. They are or were especially highly developed among the [[Warlpiri Sign Language|Warlpiri]], [[Warumungu]], [[Dieri]], [[Kaytetye]], [[Arrernte]], [[Warlmanpa]], and are based on their respective spoken languages.
 +
 +
A [[pidgin]] sign language arose among tribes of [[Native Americans in the United States|American Indians]] in the [[Great Plains]] region of [[North America]] (see [[Plains Indian Sign Language]]). It was used to communicate among tribes with different spoken [[language]]s. There are especially users today among the [[Crow tribe|Crow]], [[Cheyenne]], and [[Arapaho]].  Unlike other sign languages developed by hearing people, it shares the spatial grammar of deaf sign languages.
  
 
===Spatial grammar and simultaneity===
 
===Spatial grammar and simultaneity===
 
Sign languages exploit the unique features of the visual medium.  
 
Sign languages exploit the unique features of the visual medium.  
Oral language is linear. Only one sound can be made or received at a time. Sign language, on the other hand, is visual; hence a whole scene can be taken in at once. Information can be loaded into several channels and expressed simultaneously. As an illustration, in English one could utter the phrase, "I drove here". To add information about the drive, one would have to make a longer phrase or even add a second, such as, "I drove here along a winding road," or "I drove here. It was a nice drive." However, in American Sign Language, information about the shape of the road or the pleasing nature of the drive can be conveyed simultaneously with the verb 'drive' by inflecting the motion of the hand, or by taking advantage of non-manual signals such as body posture and facial expression, at the same time that the verb 'drive' is being signed. Therefore, whereas in English the phrase "I drove here and it was very pleasant" is longer than "I drove here", in American Sign Language the two may be the same length.
+
Oral language is linear. Only one sound can be made or received at a time. Sign language, on the other hand, is visual; hence a whole scene can be taken in at once. Information can be loaded into several channels and expressed simultaneously. As an illustration, in English one could utter the phrase, "I drove here." To add information about the drive, one would have to make a longer phrase or even add a second, such as, "I drove here along a winding road," or "I drove here. It was a nice drive." However, in American Sign Language, information about the shape of the road or the pleasing nature of the drive can be conveyed simultaneously with the verb 'drive' by inflecting the motion of the hand, or by taking advantage of non-manual signals such as body posture and facial expression, at the same time that the verb 'drive' is being signed. Therefore, whereas in English the phrase "I drove here and it was very pleasant" is longer than "I drove here," in American Sign Language the two may be the same length.
  
In fact, in terms of syntax, ASL shares more with spoken Japanese than it does with English.(Karen Nakamura,1995)
+
In fact, in terms of syntax, ASL shares more with spoken [[Japanese grammar|Japanese]] than it does with English.(Karen Nakamura,1995)
  
 
===Written forms of sign languages===
 
===Written forms of sign languages===
[[Image: Signwriting_vgt_fotomodel.png |thumb|A sign for ''"photo model"'' using [[SignWriting]] in the dictionary of the [[Flemish Sign Language]]]]Sign language differs from oral language in its relation to writing. The [[phoneme|phonemic]] systems of oral languages are primarily ''sequential'': that is, the majority of phonemes are produced in a sequence one after another, although many languages also have non-sequential aspects such as tone. As a consequence, traditional phonemic writing systems are also sequential, with at best [[diacritic]]s for non-sequential aspects such as stress and tone.  
+
Sign language differs from oral language in its relation to writing. The [[phoneme|phonemic]] systems of oral languages are primarily ''sequential'': that is, the majority of phonemes are produced in a sequence one after another, although many languages also have non-sequential aspects such as tone. As a consequence, traditional phonemic writing systems are also sequential, with at best [[diacritic]]s for non-sequential aspects such as stress and tone.  
  
 
Sign languages have a higher non-sequential component, with many "phonemes" produced simultaneously. For example, signs may involve fingers, hands, and face moving simultaneously, or the two hands moving in different directions. Traditional writing systems are not designed to deal with this level of complexity.  
 
Sign languages have a higher non-sequential component, with many "phonemes" produced simultaneously. For example, signs may involve fingers, hands, and face moving simultaneously, or the two hands moving in different directions. Traditional writing systems are not designed to deal with this level of complexity.  
  
Partially because of this, sign languages are not often written. Most deaf signers read and write the oral language of their country. However, there have been several attempts at developing scripts for sign language. These have included both "phonetic" systems, such as [http://www.sign-lang.uni-hamburg.de/Projects/HamNoSys.html HamNoSys] (the Hamburg Notational System) and [[SignWriting]], which can be used for any sign language, and "phonemic" systems such as the one used by [[William Stokoe]] in his [[1965]] ''Dictionary of American Sign Language'', which are designed for a specific language.
+
Partially because of this, sign languages are not often written. Most deaf signers read and write the oral language of their country. However, there have been several attempts at developing scripts for sign language. These have included both "phonetic" systems, such as [http://www.sign-lang.uni-hamburg.de/Projects/HamNoSys.html HamNoSys] (the Hamburg Notational System) and [[SignWriting]], which can be used for any sign language, and "phonemic" systems such as the one used by [[William Stokoe]] in his 1965 ''Dictionary of American Sign Language'', which are designed for a specific language.
  
These systems are based on [[iconic]] [[symbol]]s. Some, such as SignWriting and HamNoSys, are [[pictograph|pictographic]], being conventionalized pictures of the hands, face, and body; others, such as the [[Stokoe notation]], are more iconic. Stokoe used letters of the Latin alphabet and Hindu numerals to indicate the handshapes used in fingerspelling, such as 'A' for a closed fist, 'B' for a flat hand, and '5' for a spread hand; but non-alphabetic symbols for location and movement, such as '[]' for the trunk of the body, '×' for contact, and '^' for an upward movement. Lloyd Anderson has gone further and attempted to write ASL using only the Latin alphabet, but has not published his work.  David J. Peterson has also attempted to create a phonetic transcription system for signing that is ASCII-friendly known as the [http://dedalvs.free.fr/slipa.html Sign Language International Phonetic Alphabet (SLIPA)].
+
These systems are based on [[iconic]] [[symbol]]s. Some, such as SignWriting and HamNoSys, are [[pictograph]]ic, being conventionalized pictures of the hands, face, and body; others, such as the [[Stokoe notation]], are more iconic. Stokoe used letters of the Latin alphabet and Arabic numerals to indicate the handshapes used in fingerspelling, such as 'A' for a closed fist, 'B' for a flat hand, and '5' for a spread hand; but non-alphabetic symbols for location and movement, such as '[]' for the trunk of the body, '×' for contact, and '^' for an upward movement. Lloyd Anderson has gone further and attempted to write ASL using only the Latin alphabet, but has not published his work.  David J. Peterson has also attempted to create a phonetic transcription system for signing that is ASCII-friendly known as the [http://dedalvs.free.fr/slipa.html Sign Language International Phonetic Alphabet (SLIPA)].
  
 
SignWriting, being pictographic, is able to represent simultaneous elements in a single sign. The Stokoe notation, on the other hand, is sequential, with a conventionalized order of a symbol for the location of the sign, then one for the hand shape, and finally one (or more) for the movement. The orientation of the hand is indicated with an optional diacritic before the hand shape. When two movements occur simultaneously, they are written one atop the other; when sequential, they are written one after the other. Neither the Stokoe nor HamNoSys scripts are designed to represent facial expressions or non-manual movements, both of which SignWriting accommodates easily, although this is being gradually corrected in HamNoSys.
 
SignWriting, being pictographic, is able to represent simultaneous elements in a single sign. The Stokoe notation, on the other hand, is sequential, with a conventionalized order of a symbol for the location of the sign, then one for the hand shape, and finally one (or more) for the movement. The orientation of the hand is indicated with an optional diacritic before the hand shape. When two movements occur simultaneously, they are written one atop the other; when sequential, they are written one after the other. Neither the Stokoe nor HamNoSys scripts are designed to represent facial expressions or non-manual movements, both of which SignWriting accommodates easily, although this is being gradually corrected in HamNoSys.
  
 
==Home sign==
 
==Home sign==
:''See main article: [[Home sign]]''
+
 
 
Sign systems are sometimes developed within a single family. For instance, when hearing parents with no sign language skills have a deaf child, an informal system of signs will naturally develop, unless repressed by the parents. The term for these mini-languages is [[home sign]] (sometimes homesign or kitchen sign).
 
Sign systems are sometimes developed within a single family. For instance, when hearing parents with no sign language skills have a deaf child, an informal system of signs will naturally develop, unless repressed by the parents. The term for these mini-languages is [[home sign]] (sometimes homesign or kitchen sign).
  
Home sign arises due to the absence of any other way to communicate. Within the span of a single lifetime and without the support or feedback of a community, the child is forced to invent signals to facilitate the meeting of his or her communication needs. Although this kind of system is grossly inadequate for the intellectual development of a child and it comes nowhere near meeting the standards linguists use to describe a complete language, it is a common occurrence.
+
Home sign arises due to the absence of any other way to communicate. Within the span of a single lifetime and without the support or feedback of a community, the child is forced to invent signals to facilitate the meeting of his or her communication needs. Although this kind of system is grossly inadequate for the intellectual development of a child and it comes nowhere near meeting the standards linguists use to describe a complete language, it is a common occurrence. No type of Home Sign is recognized as an official language.
  
==Media==
+
==Benefits==
 +
In the academic realm, if a deaf student has a strong foundation in American Sign Language, his/her ability to develop English literacy skills improves. Colleges now recognize that it is possible for many deaf students to be literate in both languages and that they need to work with knowledgeable and skilled professionals in order to do so. Research has shown that mastery in language, and specifically in American Sign Language yields many benefits. One benefit is that the child is instilled  with a sense of cultural identity, which enables them to bond with other deaf individuals. This then leads to greater self-esteem and a curiosity of the world, both of which will enrich the student academically and socially.{{Fact|date=March 2007}}
  
{{video|filename=LSF_Vocab_demander.ogg|title=''Demander'' in French Sign Language|description=A demonstration of the [[French Sign Language]] word [[wiktionary:Demander#French|demander]] (verb - ''to ask'').|format=[[Ogg]]}}
+
==Notes==
 +
<references/>
  
==Benefits==
+
==References==
 +
 
 +
* Branson, J., D. Miller, & I G. Marsaja. (1996). "Everyone here speaks sign language, too: a deaf village in Bali, Indonesia." In: C. Lucas (ed.): Multicultural aspects of sociolinguistics in deaf communities. Washington, Gallaudet University Press, pp. 39-5
 +
*  Emmorey, Karen; & Lane, Harlan L. (Eds.). (2000). ''The signs of language revisited: An anthology to honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima''. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-3246-7.
 +
* Groce, Nora E. (1988). ''Everyone here spoke sign language: Hereditary deafness on Martha's Vineyard''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-27041-X.
 +
* Kendon, Adam. (1988). ''Sign Languages of Aboriginal Australia: Cultural, Semiotic and Communicative Perspectives.'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
 +
* Klima, Edward S.; & Bellugi, Ursula. (1979). ''The signs of language''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-80795-2.
 +
* Krzywkowska, Grazyna (2006). [http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Image:Hallatlan_article_final_Grazyna_Krzywkowska_2006.pdf "Przede wszystkim komunikacja"], an article about a dictionary of Hungarian sign language on the internet, in Polish.
 +
* Lane, Harlan L. (Ed.). (1984). ''The Deaf experience: Classics in language and education''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-19460-8.
 +
* Lane, Harlan L. (1984). ''When the mind hears: A history of the deaf''. New York: Random House. ISBN 0-394-50878-5.
 +
* Padden, Carol; &  Humphries, Tom. (1988). ''Deaf in America: Voices from a culture''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-19423-3.
 +
* Poizner, Howard; Klima, Edward S.; & Bellugi, Ursula. (1987). ''What the hands reveal about the brain''. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
 +
* Sacks, Oliver W. (1989). ''Seeing voices: A journey into the land of the deaf''. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-06083-0.
 +
* Sandler, Wendy; & Lillo-Martin, Diane. (2001). Natural sign languages. In M. Aronoff & J. Rees-Miller (Eds.), ''Handbook of linguistics'' (pp. 533-562). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. ISBN 0-631-20497-0.
 +
* Stiles-Davis, Joan; Kritchevsky, Mark; & Bellugi, Ursula (Eds.). (1988). ''Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development''. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-0046-8; ISBN 0-8058-0078-6.
 +
* Stokoe, William C. (1960). ''Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf''. Studies in linguistics: Occasional papers (No. 8). Buffalo: Dept. of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo.
  
In the academic realm, if a deaf student has a strong
 
foundation in American Sign Language, his/her ability
 
to develop English literacy skills improves. Colleagues now
 
recognize that it is possible for many deaf students
 
to be bilingual in both languages and that they need
 
to work with knowledgeable and skilled professionals
 
in order to do so. Research has shown that mastery in
 
language, and specifically in American Sign Language
 
yields many benefits. One benefit is that the child is
 
instilled  with a sense of cultural identity,which
 
enables them to bond with other deaf individuals. This
 
then leads to greater self-esteem and a curiosity of
 
the world, both of which will enrich the student
 
academically and socially.
 
  
==See also==
 
*[[Body language]]
 
*[[Baby Sign]] (teaching infants sign language before they have the ability to speak)
 
*[[Chinese number gestures]]
 
*[[Legal recognition of sign languages]] (status per country/region)
 
*[[List of sign languages]]
 
*[[Intercultural competence]]
 
*[[Metacommunicative competence]]
 
*[[Nonverbal communication]]
 
  
 
== External links ==
 
== External links ==
{{commonscat|Sign language}}
+
 
 +
<br style="clear:both">
 +
 
 
*[http://asl.gs/ ABC Slider] Learn ASL fingerspelling fast and easy using this free tool.
 
*[http://asl.gs/ ABC Slider] Learn ASL fingerspelling fast and easy using this free tool.
*[http://www.about-down-syndrome.com/sign-language.html About Down Syndrome]  Teach your baby sign language
+
*[http://www.lifeprint.com/ ASL Lesson Plans] Free online lessons, ASL dictionary, and lots of resources.
*[http://www.childstoryhour.com/signlanguage.htm American Sign Language]  Teach your kids american sign language
 
 
 
*[http://www.assistech.com/asl.htm ASL Books and CDs] Resources to learn and polish up your ASL skills.
 
*[http://www.lifeprint.com/asl101/ ASL Resource Site] Free online lessons and ASL dictionary.
 
 
* [http://commtechlab.msu.edu/sites/aslweb/browser.htm ASL Browser] (video of thousands of ASL signs)
 
* [http://commtechlab.msu.edu/sites/aslweb/browser.htm ASL Browser] (video of thousands of ASL signs)
*[http://www.asl.ms/ ASL Fingerspelling Online Practice Tool] Test and improve your receptive fingerspelling skills using this free online resource.
+
*[http://www.asl.ms/ ASL Fingerspelling Online Practice Tool] Test and improve your receptive fingerspelling skills.
 +
*[http://www.aslpro.com/ ASL Pro] completely free online ASL educational resource website featuring over 11000 ASL Signs.
 +
*http://www.auslan.org.au/index.cfm
 
*[http://www.british-sign.co.uk British Sign Language] Resources and information.  Also Online Course and CDROM.
 
*[http://www.british-sign.co.uk British Sign Language] Resources and information.  Also Online Course and CDROM.
 
*[http://www.learnbsl.org/ BSL animated dictionary] including mathematics in BSL
 
*[http://www.learnbsl.org/ BSL animated dictionary] including mathematics in BSL
Line 121: Line 128:
 
* [http://www.signpostbsl.com/ BSL signing services] for broadcast and various other media, part of [[ITV]]
 
* [http://www.signpostbsl.com/ BSL signing services] for broadcast and various other media, part of [[ITV]]
 
*[http://www.rnid.org.uk/bsl/ BSL video clips] at the [[RNID]]
 
*[http://www.rnid.org.uk/bsl/ BSL video clips] at the [[RNID]]
 +
* [http://www.coloroflanguage.com Color of Language]  Sign Language Info and Resources (non-profit)
 
* [http://www.deaf247.co.uk Deaf 24/7] British Sign Language and deafness related information especially in United Kingdom.
 
* [http://www.deaf247.co.uk Deaf 24/7] British Sign Language and deafness related information especially in United Kingdom.
 
* [http://www.johnlubotsky.com/deafcinema/ Deaf Cinema List] Films in ASL and other sign languages
 
* [http://www.johnlubotsky.com/deafcinema/ Deaf Cinema List] Films in ASL and other sign languages
Line 127: Line 135:
 
* [http://www.dictionaryofsign.com/ Dictionary of Sign] A wiki style user compiled dictionary of sign language (Various Countries)
 
* [http://www.dictionaryofsign.com/ Dictionary of Sign] A wiki style user compiled dictionary of sign language (Various Countries)
 
* [http://www.g-home.hu/hallatlan_new/index.php?lang=ENG Dictionary of Hungarian Sign Language (with English Translation)]
 
* [http://www.g-home.hu/hallatlan_new/index.php?lang=ENG Dictionary of Hungarian Sign Language (with English Translation)]
 +
* [http://www.discoverahobby.com/learnsignlanguage.htm Discover a Hobby: Free Online guide for learning Sign Language]
 
* [http://www.handspeak.com/ Handspeak] sign Language dictionary and more
 
* [http://www.handspeak.com/ Handspeak] sign Language dictionary and more
 
* [http://listserv.linguistlist.org/archives/slling-l.html List Serv for Sign Language Linguistics]
 
* [http://listserv.linguistlist.org/archives/slling-l.html List Serv for Sign Language Linguistics]
Line 138: Line 147:
 
* [http://www.gutenberg.org/files/17451/17451-h/17451-h.htm Sign language among North American Indians compared with that among other peoples and deaf-mutes], by Garrick Mallery from [[Project Gutenberg]]. A first annual report of the Bureau of Ethnology to the Secretary of the [[Smithsonian Institution]], 1879-1880
 
* [http://www.gutenberg.org/files/17451/17451-h/17451-h.htm Sign language among North American Indians compared with that among other peoples and deaf-mutes], by Garrick Mallery from [[Project Gutenberg]]. A first annual report of the Bureau of Ethnology to the Secretary of the [[Smithsonian Institution]], 1879-1880
  
==Further reading ==
+
* [http://www.asd-1817.org/history/history-asd.html History of sign language in the United States] (American School for the Deaf Website).
* Branson, J., D. Miller, & I G. Marsaja. 1996. "Everyone here speaks sign language, too: a deaf village in Bali, Indonesia." In: C. Lucas (ed.): Multicultural aspects of sociolinguistics in deaf communities. Washington, Gallaudet University Press, pp. 39-5
+
* [http://www.rnid.org.uk/about/rnid_history/ History of the Royal National Institute for Deaf People] (sign language in the [[United Kingdom|UK]]).
*  Emmorey, Karen; & Lane, Harlan L. (Eds.). (2000). ''The signs of language revisited: An anthology to honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima''. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-3246-7.
+
*[http://www.lifeprint.com/asl101/pages-layout/history1.htm American Sign Language (ASL) History Lesson]
* Groce, Nora E. (1988). ''Everyone here spoke sign language: Hereditary deafness on Martha's Vineyard''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-27041-X.
+
 
* Kendon, Adam. 1988. ''Sign Languages of Aboriginal Australia: Cultural, Semiotic and Communicative Perspectives.'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
 
* Klima, Edward S.; & Bellugi, Ursula. (1979). ''The signs of language''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-80795-2.
 
* Lane, Harlan L. (Ed.). (1984). ''The Deaf experience: Classics in language and education''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-19460-8.
 
* Lane, Harlan L. (1984). ''When the mind hears: A history of the deaf''. New York: Random House. ISBN 0-394-50878-5.
 
* Padden, Carol; &  Humphries, Tom. (1988). ''Deaf in America: Voices from a culture''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-19423-3.
 
* Poizner, Howard; Klima, Edward S.; & Bellugi, Ursula. (1987). ''What the hands reveal about the brain''. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
 
* Sacks, Oliver W. (1989). ''Seeing voices: A journey into the land of the deaf''. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-06083-0.
 
* Sandler, Wendy; & Lillo-Martin, Diane. (2001). Natural sign languages. In M. Aronoff & J. Rees-Miller (Eds.), ''Handbook of linguistics'' (pp. 533-562). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. ISBN 0-631-20497-0.
 
* Stiles-Davis, Joan; Kritchevsky, Mark; & Bellugi, Ursula (Eds.). (1988). ''Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development''. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-0046-8; ISBN 0-8058-0078-6.
 
* Stokoe, William C. (1960). ''Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf''. Studies in linguistics: Occasional papers (No. 8). Buffalo: Dept. of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo.
 
  
  
{{Credit1|Sign_language|94216823|}}
+
{{Credits|Sign_language|153605140|History_of_sign_language|116881318|}}

Revision as of 17:54, 29 August 2007


Two sign language Intepreters working as a team for a school.

A sign language (also signed language) is a language which uses manual communication, body language and lip patterns instead of sound to convey meaning—simultaneously combining hand shapes, orientation and movement of the hands, arms or body, and facial expressions to express fluidly a speaker's thoughts. Sign languages commonly develop in deaf communities, which can include interpreters and friends and families of deaf people as well as people who are deaf or hard of hearing themselves.

As is the case in spoken language, sign language differs from one region to another. However, when people using different signed languages meet, communication is significantly easier than when people of different spoken languages meet.[citation needed] Sign language, in this respect, gives access to an international deaf community. Sign language is however not universal, and many different sign languages exist that are mostly mutually unintelligible.

Wherever communities of deaf people exist, sign languages develop, in fact their complex spatial grammars are markedly different than spoken language. In many cases, various signed "modes" of spoken languages have been developed, such as Signed English and Warlpiri Sign Language. Hundreds of sign languages are in use around the world and are at the core of local Deaf cultures. Some sign languages have obtained some form of legal recognition, while others have no status at all.

Exemplary for the mature status of sign languages is the growing body of sign language poetry, and other stage performances. The poetic mechanisms available to signing poets are not all available to a speaking poet. This offers new, exciting ways for poems to reach and move the audience.

History of sign language

The recorded history of sign language in Western society extends from the 16th century. In 1755, Abbé de l'Épée founded the first public school for deaf children in Paris; Laurent Clerc was arguably its most famous graduate. He went to the United States with Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet to found the American School for the Deaf in Hartford, Connecticut.[1] Gallaudet's son, Edward Miner Gallaudet founded the first college for the deaf in 1857, which in 1864 became Gallaudet University, the only liberal arts university for the deaf in the world. Gallaudet University is still present today; it is located in Washington DC.

Generally, each spoken language has a sign language counterpart in as much as each linguistic population will contain Deaf members who will generate a sign language. In much the same way that geographical or cultural forces will isolate populations and lead to the generation of different and distinct spoken languages, the same forces operate on signed languages and so they tend to maintain their identities through time in roughly the same areas of influence as the local spoken languages. This occurs even though sign languages have no relation to the spoken languages of the lands in which they arise. There are notable exceptions to this pattern, however, as some geographic regions sharing a spoken language have multiple, unrelated signed languages.

Variations within a 'national' sign language can usually be correlated to the geographic location of (residential) schools for the deaf.

International Sign, formerly known as Gestuno, is used mainly at international Deaf events such as the Deaflympics and meetings of the World Federation of the Deaf. Recent studies claim that while International Sign is a kind of a pidgin, they conclude that it is more complex than a typical pidgin and indeed is more like a full signed language.

The recorded history of sign language in Western society extends from the 16th century, as a visual language or method of communication. Sign language is a system of conventional gestures, mimic, hand signs and finger spelling, the use of hand positions to represent the letters of the alphabet. Signs usually represent complete ideas, not only words. Sign language is mainly used by deaf and mute people in order to communicate.

Development of Sign Language

Standardized sign languages have been used in Italy since the 17th century and in France since the 18th century for the instruction of the deaf. Old French Sign Language was developed and used in Paris by the l’Abbé Charles Michel de l’Epée in his school for the deaf. These languages were always modeled after the natural sign languages already in use by the deaf cultures in their area of origin, often with additions to show aspects of the grammar of the local spoken languages.

In 1755, Abbé de l'Épée founded the first public school for deaf children in Paris. His lessons were based upon his observations of deaf people signing with hands in the streets of Paris. Synthesized with French grammar, it evolved into the French Sign Language. Laurent Clerc, a graduate and former teacher of the French School, went to the United States with Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet to found an American School for the deaf.

The 18th permanent school for the deaf was established in Hartford, Connecticut; others followed. In 1817, Clerc and Gallaudet founded the American Asylum for the Deaf and Dumb (now the American School for the Deaf). In 1864, a college for deaf people was founded in Washington D.C. Its charter was signed by Abraham Lincoln and was named “Gallaudet College.”


Linguistics of sign

In linguistic terms, sign languages are as rich and complex as any oral language, despite the common misconception that they are not "real languages." Professional linguists have studied many sign languages and found them to have every linguistic component required to be classed as true languages.

Sign languages are not pantomime - in other words, signs are largely arbitrary and have no necessary visual relationship to their referent, much as most spoken language is not onomatopoeic. Nor are they a visual rendition of an oral language. They have complex grammars of their own, and can be used to discuss any topic, from the simple and concrete to the lofty and abstract.

Sign languages, like oral languages, organize elementary, meaningless units (phonemes; once called cheremes in the case of sign languages) into meaningful semantic units. The elements of a sign are Handshape (or Handform), Orientation (or Palm Orientation), Location (or Place of Articulation), Movement, and Non-manual markers (or Facial Expression), summarised in the acronym HOLME.

Common linguistic features of deaf sign languages are extensive use of classifiers, a high degree of inflection, and a topic-comment syntax. Many unique linguistic features emerge from sign languages' ability to produce meaning in different parts of the visual field simultaneously. For example, the recipient of a signed message can read meanings carried by the hands, the facial expression and the body posture in the same moment. This is in contrast to oral languages, where the sounds that comprise words are mostly sequential (tone being an exception).

Sign languages' relationships with oral languages

A common misconception is that sign languages are somehow dependent on oral languages, that is, that they are oral language spelled out in gesture, or that they were invented by hearing people. Hearing teachers of deaf schools, such as Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet, are often incorrectly referred to as inventors of sign language.

The manual alphabet is used in sign languages, mostly for proper names and technical or specialised vocabulary. The use of fingerspelling was once taken as evidence that sign languages are simplified versions of oral languages, but in fact it is merely one tool among many. Fingerspelling can sometimes be a source of new signs, which are called lexicalized signs.

On the whole, deaf sign languages are independent of oral languages and follow their own paths of development. For example, British Sign Language and American Sign Language are quite different and mutually unintelligible, even though the hearing people of Britain and America share the same oral language.

Similarly, countries which use a single oral language throughout may have two or more sign languages; whereas an area that contains more than one oral language might use only one sign language.

Use of signs in hearing communities

Gesture is a typical component of spoken languages. More elaborate systems of manual communication have developed in situations where speech is not practical or permitted, such as cloistered religious communities, scuba diving, television recording studios, loud workplaces, stock exchanges, in baseball, while hunting (by groups such as the Kalahari bushmen), or in the game Charades. In Rugby Union the Referee uses a limited but defined set of signs to communicate his/her decisions to the spectators. Recently, there has been a movement to teach and encourage the use of sign language with toddlers before they learn to talk and with non-deaf or hard-of-hearing children with other causes of speech impairment or delay. This is typically referred to as Baby Sign.

File:Jw signs convention.jpg
Jehovah's Witnesses Convention in Chilean Sign Language.

On occasion, where the prevalence of deaf people is high enough, a deaf sign language has been taken up by an entire local community. Famous examples of this include Martha's Vineyard Sign Language in the USA, Kata Kolok in a village in Bali, Adamorobe Sign Language in Ghana and Yucatec Maya sign language in Mexico. In such communities deaf people are not socially disadvantaged.

Many Australian Aboriginal sign languages arose in a context of extensive speech taboos, such as during mourning and initiation rites. They are or were especially highly developed among the Warlpiri, Warumungu, Dieri, Kaytetye, Arrernte, Warlmanpa, and are based on their respective spoken languages.

A pidgin sign language arose among tribes of American Indians in the Great Plains region of North America (see Plains Indian Sign Language). It was used to communicate among tribes with different spoken languages. There are especially users today among the Crow, Cheyenne, and Arapaho. Unlike other sign languages developed by hearing people, it shares the spatial grammar of deaf sign languages.

Spatial grammar and simultaneity

Sign languages exploit the unique features of the visual medium. Oral language is linear. Only one sound can be made or received at a time. Sign language, on the other hand, is visual; hence a whole scene can be taken in at once. Information can be loaded into several channels and expressed simultaneously. As an illustration, in English one could utter the phrase, "I drove here." To add information about the drive, one would have to make a longer phrase or even add a second, such as, "I drove here along a winding road," or "I drove here. It was a nice drive." However, in American Sign Language, information about the shape of the road or the pleasing nature of the drive can be conveyed simultaneously with the verb 'drive' by inflecting the motion of the hand, or by taking advantage of non-manual signals such as body posture and facial expression, at the same time that the verb 'drive' is being signed. Therefore, whereas in English the phrase "I drove here and it was very pleasant" is longer than "I drove here," in American Sign Language the two may be the same length.

In fact, in terms of syntax, ASL shares more with spoken Japanese than it does with English.(Karen Nakamura,1995)

Written forms of sign languages

Sign language differs from oral language in its relation to writing. The phonemic systems of oral languages are primarily sequential: that is, the majority of phonemes are produced in a sequence one after another, although many languages also have non-sequential aspects such as tone. As a consequence, traditional phonemic writing systems are also sequential, with at best diacritics for non-sequential aspects such as stress and tone.

Sign languages have a higher non-sequential component, with many "phonemes" produced simultaneously. For example, signs may involve fingers, hands, and face moving simultaneously, or the two hands moving in different directions. Traditional writing systems are not designed to deal with this level of complexity.

Partially because of this, sign languages are not often written. Most deaf signers read and write the oral language of their country. However, there have been several attempts at developing scripts for sign language. These have included both "phonetic" systems, such as HamNoSys (the Hamburg Notational System) and SignWriting, which can be used for any sign language, and "phonemic" systems such as the one used by William Stokoe in his 1965 Dictionary of American Sign Language, which are designed for a specific language.

These systems are based on iconic symbols. Some, such as SignWriting and HamNoSys, are pictographic, being conventionalized pictures of the hands, face, and body; others, such as the Stokoe notation, are more iconic. Stokoe used letters of the Latin alphabet and Arabic numerals to indicate the handshapes used in fingerspelling, such as 'A' for a closed fist, 'B' for a flat hand, and '5' for a spread hand; but non-alphabetic symbols for location and movement, such as '[]' for the trunk of the body, '×' for contact, and '^' for an upward movement. Lloyd Anderson has gone further and attempted to write ASL using only the Latin alphabet, but has not published his work. David J. Peterson has also attempted to create a phonetic transcription system for signing that is ASCII-friendly known as the Sign Language International Phonetic Alphabet (SLIPA).

SignWriting, being pictographic, is able to represent simultaneous elements in a single sign. The Stokoe notation, on the other hand, is sequential, with a conventionalized order of a symbol for the location of the sign, then one for the hand shape, and finally one (or more) for the movement. The orientation of the hand is indicated with an optional diacritic before the hand shape. When two movements occur simultaneously, they are written one atop the other; when sequential, they are written one after the other. Neither the Stokoe nor HamNoSys scripts are designed to represent facial expressions or non-manual movements, both of which SignWriting accommodates easily, although this is being gradually corrected in HamNoSys.

Home sign

Sign systems are sometimes developed within a single family. For instance, when hearing parents with no sign language skills have a deaf child, an informal system of signs will naturally develop, unless repressed by the parents. The term for these mini-languages is home sign (sometimes homesign or kitchen sign).

Home sign arises due to the absence of any other way to communicate. Within the span of a single lifetime and without the support or feedback of a community, the child is forced to invent signals to facilitate the meeting of his or her communication needs. Although this kind of system is grossly inadequate for the intellectual development of a child and it comes nowhere near meeting the standards linguists use to describe a complete language, it is a common occurrence. No type of Home Sign is recognized as an official language.

Benefits

In the academic realm, if a deaf student has a strong foundation in American Sign Language, his/her ability to develop English literacy skills improves. Colleges now recognize that it is possible for many deaf students to be literate in both languages and that they need to work with knowledgeable and skilled professionals in order to do so. Research has shown that mastery in language, and specifically in American Sign Language yields many benefits. One benefit is that the child is instilled with a sense of cultural identity, which enables them to bond with other deaf individuals. This then leads to greater self-esteem and a curiosity of the world, both of which will enrich the student academically and socially.[citation needed]

Notes

  1. Canlas, Loida (2006-07-10). Laurent Clerc: Apostle to the Deaf People of the New World. Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Center at Gallaudet University. Retrieved 2007-01-06.

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Branson, J., D. Miller, & I G. Marsaja. (1996). "Everyone here speaks sign language, too: a deaf village in Bali, Indonesia." In: C. Lucas (ed.): Multicultural aspects of sociolinguistics in deaf communities. Washington, Gallaudet University Press, pp. 39-5
  • Emmorey, Karen; & Lane, Harlan L. (Eds.). (2000). The signs of language revisited: An anthology to honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-3246-7.
  • Groce, Nora E. (1988). Everyone here spoke sign language: Hereditary deafness on Martha's Vineyard. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-27041-X.
  • Kendon, Adam. (1988). Sign Languages of Aboriginal Australia: Cultural, Semiotic and Communicative Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Klima, Edward S.; & Bellugi, Ursula. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-80795-2.
  • Krzywkowska, Grazyna (2006). "Przede wszystkim komunikacja", an article about a dictionary of Hungarian sign language on the internet, in Polish.
  • Lane, Harlan L. (Ed.). (1984). The Deaf experience: Classics in language and education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-19460-8.
  • Lane, Harlan L. (1984). When the mind hears: A history of the deaf. New York: Random House. ISBN 0-394-50878-5.
  • Padden, Carol; & Humphries, Tom. (1988). Deaf in America: Voices from a culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-19423-3.
  • Poizner, Howard; Klima, Edward S.; & Bellugi, Ursula. (1987). What the hands reveal about the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Sacks, Oliver W. (1989). Seeing voices: A journey into the land of the deaf. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-06083-0.
  • Sandler, Wendy; & Lillo-Martin, Diane. (2001). Natural sign languages. In M. Aronoff & J. Rees-Miller (Eds.), Handbook of linguistics (pp. 533-562). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. ISBN 0-631-20497-0.
  • Stiles-Davis, Joan; Kritchevsky, Mark; & Bellugi, Ursula (Eds.). (1988). Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-0046-8; ISBN 0-8058-0078-6.
  • Stokoe, William C. (1960). Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf. Studies in linguistics: Occasional papers (No. 8). Buffalo: Dept. of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo.


External links



Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.