Difference between revisions of "Cornsweet illusion" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
(started)
 
(17 intermediate revisions by 7 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Claimed}}{{Started}}
+
{{Copyedited}}{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}{{Approved}}{{Paid}}
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 +
[[Category:Illusion]]
 +
[[Image:Cornsweet illusion.svg|right|thumb|250px|'''Cornsweet illusion'''. The left part of the picture seems to be darker then the right one. In fact they have the same brightness.]]
  
[[Image:Cornsweet illusion.svg|right|thumb|250px|'''Cornsweet illusion'''. Left part of the picture seems to be darker then the right one. In fact they have the same brightness.]]
+
The '''Cornsweet illusion''', also known as ''Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion'' or ''Craik-Cornsweet illusion'', is an [[optical illusion]] where the gradient within a central line or section creates the impression that one side of the image is in fact darker than the other. In reality, both sections are the same.  
[[Image:Cornsweet illusion black band.svg|right|thumb|250px|The same image as above, but the edge in the middle is hidden. Left and right part of the image look of the same color now.]]
+
{{toc}}
[[Image:Cornsweet illusion explanation.svg|right|thumb|500px|The actual distribution of luminance in the picture, and the typical perception of luminance.]]
+
The Cornsweet illusion, as well as numerous other visual and perceptual [[illusion]]s, provide a valuable way to investigate how the [[eye]] and [[brain]] process visual information. Equally, they are used by [[art]]ists for visual effect, entertaining and satisfying the endless fascination human beings have with novelty and [[creativity]].
The '''Cornsweet illusion''', also known as ''Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion'' or ''Craik-Cornsweet illusion'', is an [[optical illusion]] that was described in detail by [[Tom Cornsweet]] in the late 1960s.<ref>Cornsweet T (1970) Visual Perception. New York: Academic Press.</ref> Craik and O'Brien had made earlier observations in a similar vein.
+
[[Image:Cornsweet illusion black band.svg|right|thumb|250px|The same image as above, but the edge in the middle is hidden. The left and right part of the image now look the same color.]]
 +
==Discovery==
 +
The ''Cornsweet illusion'', also known as ''Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion'' or ''Craik-Cornsweet illusion'', is an [[optical illusion]] that was described in detail by [[Tom Cornsweet]] in the late 1960s, and published in 1970 in 'Visual Perception'. Craik and O'Brien had made earlier observations in a similar vein, and thus the illusion is often referred to as the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion.<ref>K.J.W. Craik. ''The Nature of Psychology''. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1966.</ref><ref>V. O’Brien V. "Contrast by contour-enhancement." ''American Journal of Psychology'', 1959, 72:299–300.</ref>
 +
 
 +
==Description==
  
In the image at right, the entire region to the right of the "edge" in the middle looks slightly lighter than the area to the left of the edge, but in fact the brightness of both areas is exactly the same, as can be seen by blacking out the region containing the edge.
+
When looking at the classic Cornsweet illusion, two squares of differing contrast appear to be separated by a thin band of gradient. In reality, the two squares are exactly the same level of contrast. In some examples, the illusion is so convincing that the only way it can be believed is to cover over everything but the two identical colors, thus proving that they are indeed the same.
  
 
This phenomenon is similar to the familiar phenomenon of [[simultaneous contrast]] and [[Mach bands]], but differs from it in two important respects.
 
This phenomenon is similar to the familiar phenomenon of [[simultaneous contrast]] and [[Mach bands]], but differs from it in two important respects.
* In Mach bands the effect is seen only on areas that are close to the intensity gradient. In Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion, a very small area (the central "edge") affects the perception of entire large areas, portions of which are distant from the edge.
+
* In Mach bands the effect is seen only on areas that are close to the intensity gradient. In the Cornsweet illusion, a very small area (the central "edge") affects the perception of entire large areas, portions of which are distant from the edge.
 
* In the Cornsweet illusion, the region adjacent to the light part of the edge appears lighter, and the region adjacent to the dark part of the edge appears darker, just the opposite of the usual contrast effects.
 
* In the Cornsweet illusion, the region adjacent to the light part of the edge appears lighter, and the region adjacent to the dark part of the edge appears darker, just the opposite of the usual contrast effects.
  
The third image at right shows the usual explanation of the effect. A far more convincing and dramatic version of the effect can be seen in the article by Purves, Lotto, and Nundy,<ref>Purves D., Lotto R.B., Nundy S. (2002) ''[http://www.americanscientist.org/template/AssetDetail/assetid/14755/page/1|Why We See What We Do]'', [[American Scientist]] 90(3):236-243.</ref> where it is presented within a quasi-realistic image of solid, illuminated objects.<ref>[http://www.americanscientist.org/content/AMSCI/AMSCI/Image/FullImage_200341093716_148.jpg Quasi-realistic image] from Purves, Lotto, and Nundy (2002) showing a much more convincing presentation of the Cornsweet illusion.</ref> These writers give a ([[Teleology|teleological]]) explanation of this and other illusions, in which the visual system and brain are posited to generate percepts on an empirical basis that is much like a reflex. In their words, ''...[perception] accords not with the features of the retinal stimulus or the properties of the underlying objects, but with what the same or similar stimuli have typically signified in the past''.
+
==Explanation==
 +
[[Image:Cornsweet illusion explanation.svg|right|thumb|500px|The actual distribution of luminance in the picture, and the typical perception of luminance.]]
 +
The Cornsweet illusion is an example of the way the [[brain]] uses the edges of shapes to impart information to the surrounding areas, and takes place because of the way [[nerve]] impulses travel through the [[visual cortex]]. The [[retina]] interprets what it sees using certain [[luminance]] profiles. The Cornsweet image is processed with nearly identical codes, which the cortex then integrates and arrives at the same perceptual result. In other words, the brain is used to seeing things a certain way, and often interprets information within those parameters.<ref>Michael Bach. [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/lum_cobc/index.html "Craik-O;Brien-Cornsweet Illusion"] June 2002. Optical Illusions and Visual Phenomena. Retrieved October 30, 2007.</ref>
 +
 
 +
==Applications==
 +
The Cornsweet illusion has applications in [[graphic design]], where it can be used to create interesting visual effects. Awareness of the Cornsweet illusion is also useful in areas like [[radiology]]; without awareness of the Cornsweet illusion, technicians can potentially misread areas of [[x-ray]] images.<ref>Ian Maddison. [http://myweb.lsbu.ac.uk/dirt/museum/physiol.html "The Physiology of the Silhouette Sign in the Chest X-ray"] November 1994. Retrieved October 30, 2007.</ref>
  
 
== Notes ==
 
== Notes ==
Line 20: Line 31:
  
 
== References ==
 
== References ==
* Purves D, Shimpi A, Lotto RB (1999) [http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/19/19/8542 An empirical explanation of the Cornsweet effect]. J. Neurosci. 19:8542-8551.
+
* Craik, K.J.W. ''The Nature of Psychology''. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1966.
* Purves D, Lotto RB (2003) ''Why We See What We Do: An Empirical Theory of Vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
+
* Fineman, Mark. ''The Nature of Visual Illusion''. Dover Publications, 1996. ISBN 0486291057
* Purves D, Lotto RB (2004) The Cornsweet effect. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, 3rd edition Elsevier Science Publishing Co.
+
* O’Brien V. "Contrast by contour-enhancement." ''American Journal of Psychology'' 72 (1959): 299–300.
 
+
* Purves D., A. Shimpi, and R.B. Lotto. [http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/19/19/8542 An empirical explanation of the Cornsweet effect] J. Neurosci. 19 (1999): 8542-8551. Retrieved May 4, 2020.
 
+
* Purves D, and R.B. Lotto. "The Cornsweet effect." In '' Encyclopedia of Neuroscience,'' 3rd edition. Elsevier Science Publishing Co., 2004.
 +
* Purves D, and R.B. Lotto. ''Why We See What We Do: An Empirical Theory of Vision''. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, 2003.
  
 
== External links ==
 
== External links ==
* [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/lum_cobc/index.html Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet Illusion] by Michael Bach.
+
All links retrieved January 7, 2024.
 
+
* [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/lum-cobc/index.html Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet Illusion] by Michael Bach.  
  
  
 
{{Credits|Cornsweet_illusion|144889736|}}
 
{{Credits|Cornsweet_illusion|144889736|}}

Latest revision as of 03:33, 8 January 2024

Cornsweet illusion. The left part of the picture seems to be darker then the right one. In fact they have the same brightness.

The Cornsweet illusion, also known as Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion or Craik-Cornsweet illusion, is an optical illusion where the gradient within a central line or section creates the impression that one side of the image is in fact darker than the other. In reality, both sections are the same.

The Cornsweet illusion, as well as numerous other visual and perceptual illusions, provide a valuable way to investigate how the eye and brain process visual information. Equally, they are used by artists for visual effect, entertaining and satisfying the endless fascination human beings have with novelty and creativity.

The same image as above, but the edge in the middle is hidden. The left and right part of the image now look the same color.

Discovery

The Cornsweet illusion, also known as Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion or Craik-Cornsweet illusion, is an optical illusion that was described in detail by Tom Cornsweet in the late 1960s, and published in 1970 in 'Visual Perception'. Craik and O'Brien had made earlier observations in a similar vein, and thus the illusion is often referred to as the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion.[1][2]

Description

When looking at the classic Cornsweet illusion, two squares of differing contrast appear to be separated by a thin band of gradient. In reality, the two squares are exactly the same level of contrast. In some examples, the illusion is so convincing that the only way it can be believed is to cover over everything but the two identical colors, thus proving that they are indeed the same.

This phenomenon is similar to the familiar phenomenon of simultaneous contrast and Mach bands, but differs from it in two important respects.

  • In Mach bands the effect is seen only on areas that are close to the intensity gradient. In the Cornsweet illusion, a very small area (the central "edge") affects the perception of entire large areas, portions of which are distant from the edge.
  • In the Cornsweet illusion, the region adjacent to the light part of the edge appears lighter, and the region adjacent to the dark part of the edge appears darker, just the opposite of the usual contrast effects.

Explanation

The actual distribution of luminance in the picture, and the typical perception of luminance.

The Cornsweet illusion is an example of the way the brain uses the edges of shapes to impart information to the surrounding areas, and takes place because of the way nerve impulses travel through the visual cortex. The retina interprets what it sees using certain luminance profiles. The Cornsweet image is processed with nearly identical codes, which the cortex then integrates and arrives at the same perceptual result. In other words, the brain is used to seeing things a certain way, and often interprets information within those parameters.[3]

Applications

The Cornsweet illusion has applications in graphic design, where it can be used to create interesting visual effects. Awareness of the Cornsweet illusion is also useful in areas like radiology; without awareness of the Cornsweet illusion, technicians can potentially misread areas of x-ray images.[4]

Notes

  1. K.J.W. Craik. The Nature of Psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1966.
  2. V. O’Brien V. "Contrast by contour-enhancement." American Journal of Psychology, 1959, 72:299–300.
  3. Michael Bach. "Craik-O;Brien-Cornsweet Illusion" June 2002. Optical Illusions and Visual Phenomena. Retrieved October 30, 2007.
  4. Ian Maddison. "The Physiology of the Silhouette Sign in the Chest X-ray" November 1994. Retrieved October 30, 2007.

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Craik, K.J.W. The Nature of Psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1966.
  • Fineman, Mark. The Nature of Visual Illusion. Dover Publications, 1996. ISBN 0486291057
  • O’Brien V. "Contrast by contour-enhancement." American Journal of Psychology 72 (1959): 299–300.
  • Purves D., A. Shimpi, and R.B. Lotto. An empirical explanation of the Cornsweet effect J. Neurosci. 19 (1999): 8542-8551. Retrieved May 4, 2020.
  • Purves D, and R.B. Lotto. "The Cornsweet effect." In Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, 3rd edition. Elsevier Science Publishing Co., 2004.
  • Purves D, and R.B. Lotto. Why We See What We Do: An Empirical Theory of Vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, 2003.

External links

All links retrieved January 7, 2024.


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.