Difference between revisions of "Rubin vase" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
(Images OK)
 
(14 intermediate revisions by 7 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Claimed}}{{Started}}{{Contracted}}{{Images OK}}
+
{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}{{Approved}}{{Paid}}{{copyedited}}
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 +
[[Category:Illusion]]
 
[[Image:Cup or faces paradox.svg|thumb|200 px|A Rubin vase]]
 
[[Image:Cup or faces paradox.svg|thumb|200 px|A Rubin vase]]
'''Rubin's vase''' (sometimes known as the '''Rubin face''' or the '''Figure-ground vase''') is a famous set of cognitive [[optical illusion]]s developed around 1915 by the [[Denmark|Danish]] [[psychologist]] [[Edgar Rubin]].  
+
'''Rubin's vase''' (sometimes known as the '''Rubin face''' or the '''Figure-ground vase''') is a famous set of cognitive [[optical illusion]]s developed around 1915, by the [[Denmark|Danish]] [[psychologist]] [[Edgar Rubin]]. Rubin's figure-ground distinction influenced the [[Gestalt psychology|Gestalt psychologists]], such as [[Max Wertheimer]] and [[Kurt Koffka]], who discovered many similar illusions themselves. The higher-level cognitive processes involved in such [[perception|perceptual]] effects intrigued the Gestalt psychologists, who were very interested in the operational principle of the brain as holistic and parallel. Artists, including the master of ambiguous figures [[M. C. Escher]], have also utilized this type of illusory effect to introduce complex and intriguing features into their works. Thus, the Rubin's vase and similar figures provide both opportunities for valuable research into human perceptual processes and to bring joy and fascination to many through their inclusion in works of [[art]], showing humanity's endless fascination with the [[creativity|creative]] and unusual.
 
+
{{toc}}
 
==Discovery==
 
==Discovery==
The '''Rubin vase''' [[illusion]] was first developed by [[psychologist]] [[Edgar Rubin]], although similar images can be found in eighteenth century French prints.<ref>[http://psylux.psych.tu-dresden.de/i1/kaw/diverses%20Material/www.illusionworks.com/html/figure_ground.html "Figure/ground"] 1997. IllusionWorks. Retrieved October 25, 2007.</ref> The image was introduced in 1915 in Rubin's two-volume work, the Danish-language ''Synsoplevede Figurer'' ("Visual Figures"), which was very well-received; Rubin included a number of similar images, such as a black and white [[Maltese cross]] figure, but the one that became the most famous was the figure-ground vase (perhaps because the Maltese cross could also be easily interpreted as a black and white [[beach ball]].)
+
The '''Rubin vase''' [[illusion]] was first developed by [[psychologist]] [[Edgar Rubin]], although similar images can be found in eighteenth century French prints.<ref>IllusionWorks, [http://psylux.psych.tu-dresden.de/i1/kaw/diverses%20Material/www.illusionworks.com/html/figure_ground.html Figure/ground.] Retrieved October 25, 2007.</ref> The image was introduced in 1915, in Rubin's two-volume work, the Danish-language ''Synsoplevede Figurer'' ("Visual Figures"), which was very well-received; Rubin included a number of similar images, such as a black and white [[Maltese cross]] figure, but the one that became the most famous was the figure-ground vase (perhaps because the Maltese cross could also be easily interpreted as a black and white [[beach ball]].)
  
 
==Description==
 
==Description==
The illusion generally presents the viewer with a mental choice of two interpretations, each of which is valid. One interpretation is of two black [[face]]s in the foreground with a white background. The other is of a black [[vase]] in the foreground with a white background. Often, the viewer sees only one of the two valid interpretations, and only realizes the second after some time or prompting. When one attempts to simultaneously see the both interpretations, one finds that it is not possible to perceive both interpretations simultaneously, as one occludes the other.
+
The illusion generally presents the viewer with a mental choice of two interpretations, each of which is valid. One interpretation is of two black [[face]]s in the foreground with a white background. The other is of a white [[vase]] in the foreground with a black background. Often, the viewer sees only one of the two valid interpretations, and only realizes the second after some time or prompting. When one attempts to simultaneously see the both interpretations, one finds that it is not possible to perceive both interpretations simultaneously, as one occludes the other.
 +
 
 +
The illusion is somewhat similar to the "[[duck-rabbit]]" ambiguous figure described by [[Joseph Jastrow]], although in that case the figure is always seen as the object but it's identification switches back and forth from that of a [[duck]] to that of a [[rabbit]]. In the Rubin vase, the face and vase alternate between figure and ground, such that the part of the image seen as object alternates.
  
 
==Explanation==
 
==Explanation==
  
 
Rubin explained the illusion as follows:
 
Rubin explained the illusion as follows:
<blockquote>One can then state as a fundamental principle: When two fields have a common border, and one is seen as [[figure]] and the other as [[ground]], the immediate perceptual experience is characterized by a shaping effect which emerges from the common border of the fields and which operates only on one field or operates more strongly on one than on the other. <ref>Rubin, Edgar. ''Synsoplevede Figurer'', 1915</ref></blockquote>
+
<blockquote>One can then state as a fundamental principle: When two fields have a common border, and one is seen as [[figure]] and the other as [[ground]], the immediate perceptual experience is characterized by a shaping effect which emerges from the common border of the fields and which operates only on one field or operates more strongly on one than on the other.<ref>Edgar Rubin, ''Synsoplevede Figurer'' (1915). </ref></blockquote>
  
 
The [[illusion]] is an excellent and intuitive demonstration of the [[figure-ground]] distinction the [[brain]] makes during [[visual perception]]. Normally the brain classifies images by what surrounds a figure, establishing [[Distance|depth]] and other relationships. If one object surrounds another, the surrounded object is usually seen as figure, and the presumably further away (and hence background) object is the ground. In the three dimensional world, if an object such as a piece of fruit is lying on the ground, one would want to pay attention to the "figure" and not the "ground." However, when the contours are relatively equal, ambiguity starts to creep in, and the brain must begin "shaping" what it sees. Since the contours are ambiguous, the brain can alternatively interpret the black areas as figure or ground.
 
The [[illusion]] is an excellent and intuitive demonstration of the [[figure-ground]] distinction the [[brain]] makes during [[visual perception]]. Normally the brain classifies images by what surrounds a figure, establishing [[Distance|depth]] and other relationships. If one object surrounds another, the surrounded object is usually seen as figure, and the presumably further away (and hence background) object is the ground. In the three dimensional world, if an object such as a piece of fruit is lying on the ground, one would want to pay attention to the "figure" and not the "ground." However, when the contours are relatively equal, ambiguity starts to creep in, and the brain must begin "shaping" what it sees. Since the contours are ambiguous, the brain can alternatively interpret the black areas as figure or ground.
 +
 +
There is no doubt that this particular illusion occurs involves [[cognition|cognitive]] processing.  Without having information stored in the brain that contains knowledge about vases and profiles, based on past experience, these reversible interpretations would not be possible.
  
 
==Applications==
 
==Applications==
Rubin's figure-ground distinction influenced the [[Gestalt psychology]|Gestalt] psychologists, who discovered many similar illusions themselves. The involved higher-level cognitive pattern matching involved, where the overall picture determines its mental interpretation, not the net effect of the individual pieces, intrigued the Gestalt psychologists, who were very interested in the operational principle of the brain as holistic and parallel.
+
Rubin's figure-ground distinction influenced the [[Gestalt psychology|Gestalt psychologists]], who discovered many similar illusions themselves. The higher-level cognitive pattern matching involved, where the overall picture determines its mental interpretation, not the net effect of the individual pieces, intrigued the Gestalt psychologists, who were very interested in the operational principle of the brain as holistic and parallel.
 +
 
 +
This ambiguity of figure and ground, and the ability to construct objects in a reversible fashion, has intrigued artists. In particular, [[M. C. Escher]] utilized figure-ground effects, similar to the face-vase figure although not based on Rubin's discovery, in several of his works. Escher also used the same figure (alternating the original with the reversed form) to great effect, such as in his [[woodcut]] ''Day and Night'' (1938).
  
 
==Notes==
 
==Notes==
 
 
<references/>
 
<references/>
  
 
==References==
 
==References==
* Kennedy, John M. ''A Psychology of Picture Perception'' 1974, Jossey-Bass Publishers, ISBN 0-87589-204-3
+
* Fineman, Mark. 1996. ''The Nature of Visual Illusion''. Dover Publications. ISBN 0486291057
* Wade, Nicholas. ''The art and science of visual illusions'' 1982 Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd. ISBN 0-7100-0868-6
+
* Hasson, Uri, Talma Hendler, Dafna Ben Bashat, and Rafael Malach. 2001. "Vase or face? A neural correlates of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain." ''Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience'', Vol 13, No. 6, 744-753.
* Ittelson, William H. ''Visual Space Perception'' 1969, Springer Publishing Company, LOCCCN 60-15818
+
* Hoffman, Donald D. 2000. ''Visual Intelligence: How We Create What We See''. W. W. Norton. ISBN 0393319679
* Hasson, Uri, Talma Hendler, Dafna Ben Bashat, and Rafael Malach. "Vase or face? A neural correlates of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain." ''Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience'', Vol 13(6), Aug 2001. pp. 744-753. ISSN 0898-929X (Print)
+
* Ittelson, William H. 1969. ''Visual Space Perception''. Springer Publishing Company.
 +
* Kennedy, John M. 1974. ''A Psychology of Picture Perception''. Jossey-Bass Publishers. ISBN 0-87589-204-3
 +
* Wade, Nicholas. 1982. ''The Art and Science of Visual Illusions''. Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd. ISBN 0-7100-0868-6
  
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
* [http://psylux.psych.tu-dresden.de/i1/kaw/diverses%20Material/www.illusionworks.com/html/figure_ground.html Figure/Ground] Illusionworks.com article Retrieved November 16, 2007.
+
All links retrieved December 21, 2022.
 +
* [http://www.tessellations.org/eschergallery8.shtml Day and Night]
 +
 
  
 
{{Credits|Rubin_vase|100336743|}}
 
{{Credits|Rubin_vase|100336743|}}

Latest revision as of 20:58, 21 December 2022

A Rubin vase

Rubin's vase (sometimes known as the Rubin face or the Figure-ground vase) is a famous set of cognitive optical illusions developed around 1915, by the Danish psychologist Edgar Rubin. Rubin's figure-ground distinction influenced the Gestalt psychologists, such as Max Wertheimer and Kurt Koffka, who discovered many similar illusions themselves. The higher-level cognitive processes involved in such perceptual effects intrigued the Gestalt psychologists, who were very interested in the operational principle of the brain as holistic and parallel. Artists, including the master of ambiguous figures M. C. Escher, have also utilized this type of illusory effect to introduce complex and intriguing features into their works. Thus, the Rubin's vase and similar figures provide both opportunities for valuable research into human perceptual processes and to bring joy and fascination to many through their inclusion in works of art, showing humanity's endless fascination with the creative and unusual.

Discovery

The Rubin vase illusion was first developed by psychologist Edgar Rubin, although similar images can be found in eighteenth century French prints.[1] The image was introduced in 1915, in Rubin's two-volume work, the Danish-language Synsoplevede Figurer ("Visual Figures"), which was very well-received; Rubin included a number of similar images, such as a black and white Maltese cross figure, but the one that became the most famous was the figure-ground vase (perhaps because the Maltese cross could also be easily interpreted as a black and white beach ball.)

Description

The illusion generally presents the viewer with a mental choice of two interpretations, each of which is valid. One interpretation is of two black faces in the foreground with a white background. The other is of a white vase in the foreground with a black background. Often, the viewer sees only one of the two valid interpretations, and only realizes the second after some time or prompting. When one attempts to simultaneously see the both interpretations, one finds that it is not possible to perceive both interpretations simultaneously, as one occludes the other.

The illusion is somewhat similar to the "duck-rabbit" ambiguous figure described by Joseph Jastrow, although in that case the figure is always seen as the object but it's identification switches back and forth from that of a duck to that of a rabbit. In the Rubin vase, the face and vase alternate between figure and ground, such that the part of the image seen as object alternates.

Explanation

Rubin explained the illusion as follows:

One can then state as a fundamental principle: When two fields have a common border, and one is seen as figure and the other as ground, the immediate perceptual experience is characterized by a shaping effect which emerges from the common border of the fields and which operates only on one field or operates more strongly on one than on the other.[2]

The illusion is an excellent and intuitive demonstration of the figure-ground distinction the brain makes during visual perception. Normally the brain classifies images by what surrounds a figure, establishing depth and other relationships. If one object surrounds another, the surrounded object is usually seen as figure, and the presumably further away (and hence background) object is the ground. In the three dimensional world, if an object such as a piece of fruit is lying on the ground, one would want to pay attention to the "figure" and not the "ground." However, when the contours are relatively equal, ambiguity starts to creep in, and the brain must begin "shaping" what it sees. Since the contours are ambiguous, the brain can alternatively interpret the black areas as figure or ground.

There is no doubt that this particular illusion occurs involves cognitive processing. Without having information stored in the brain that contains knowledge about vases and profiles, based on past experience, these reversible interpretations would not be possible.

Applications

Rubin's figure-ground distinction influenced the Gestalt psychologists, who discovered many similar illusions themselves. The higher-level cognitive pattern matching involved, where the overall picture determines its mental interpretation, not the net effect of the individual pieces, intrigued the Gestalt psychologists, who were very interested in the operational principle of the brain as holistic and parallel.

This ambiguity of figure and ground, and the ability to construct objects in a reversible fashion, has intrigued artists. In particular, M. C. Escher utilized figure-ground effects, similar to the face-vase figure although not based on Rubin's discovery, in several of his works. Escher also used the same figure (alternating the original with the reversed form) to great effect, such as in his woodcut Day and Night (1938).

Notes

  1. IllusionWorks, Figure/ground. Retrieved October 25, 2007.
  2. Edgar Rubin, Synsoplevede Figurer (1915).

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Fineman, Mark. 1996. The Nature of Visual Illusion. Dover Publications. ISBN 0486291057
  • Hasson, Uri, Talma Hendler, Dafna Ben Bashat, and Rafael Malach. 2001. "Vase or face? A neural correlates of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol 13, No. 6, 744-753.
  • Hoffman, Donald D. 2000. Visual Intelligence: How We Create What We See. W. W. Norton. ISBN 0393319679
  • Ittelson, William H. 1969. Visual Space Perception. Springer Publishing Company.
  • Kennedy, John M. 1974. A Psychology of Picture Perception. Jossey-Bass Publishers. ISBN 0-87589-204-3
  • Wade, Nicholas. 1982. The Art and Science of Visual Illusions. Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd. ISBN 0-7100-0868-6

External links

All links retrieved December 21, 2022.


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.