Difference between revisions of "Public opinion" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
Line 18: Line 18:
  
 
American sociolologist [[Herbert Blumer]] proposed a somewhat different conception of the "public," as a form of collective behavior (another specialized term) which is made up of those who are discussing a given public issue at any one time.  Given this definition, there are many publics; each of them comes into being when an issue arises and ceases to exist when the issue is resolved. Blumer claimed that since people participate in a public to different degrees, public opinion polling cannot measure the public:  An archbishop's participation is more important than that of a bum. The "mass," in which people independently make decisions about, for example, which brand of toothpaste to buy, is a form of collective behavior different from the public.
 
American sociolologist [[Herbert Blumer]] proposed a somewhat different conception of the "public," as a form of collective behavior (another specialized term) which is made up of those who are discussing a given public issue at any one time.  Given this definition, there are many publics; each of them comes into being when an issue arises and ceases to exist when the issue is resolved. Blumer claimed that since people participate in a public to different degrees, public opinion polling cannot measure the public:  An archbishop's participation is more important than that of a bum. The "mass," in which people independently make decisions about, for example, which brand of toothpaste to buy, is a form of collective behavior different from the public.
 +
 +
==Formation of Public Opinion==
 +
Public opinion is a strange, fickle creature. Many things influence the constitution of public thought, sometimes seemingly at random. The mass media, word of mouth, economy, sense of community, advertising, and propaganda all have some effect on public opinion. [[Paul Lazarsfeld]] argued that the public forms its opinion in a two-stage process. He thought most people rely on opinion leaders. These opinion leaders are affected by world events and then pass opinions down to less active members of society. There are other contributors to the shaping of public opinion, however.
 +
 +
Lazarsfeld believed that the mass media was the main source of information for opinion leaders, but his theory may have missed the tremendous impact the mass media has over every citizen, not just a select few. Most people gather all of their information regarding current events from some outlet of the mass media be it large newspapers, television news, or the internet. The information these people retain is largely colored by the opinions of those presenting them. As a result, many people take on the opinions of their news presenters (although one could also argue that they gravitate to those broadcast outlets because of similar shared opinions).
  
 
==Mass media and public opinion==
 
==Mass media and public opinion==
Line 37: Line 42:
 
There are also empirical problems with many of these early surveys, with researchers often ignoring important findings which would ascribe significant influence to the media (eg Lazarsfeld, Berelson and Gaudet note in ''The People’s Choice'' that 58% of voting changes were made without any remembered personal contact and were very often dependent on the mass media - changes being widely distributed among those who changed their opinion. But this effect was ignored in their conclusion of little direct media influence). Other studies supporting the opinion leader theory failed to distinguish between opinion leading in consumer and political behaviour. In political behaviour opinion leading tends to correlate positively with status, whereas this is not the case in consumer behaviour (breakfast cereals etc). So for political behaviour, the general conclusion that the media merely fixes (confirms) people’s opinion is not supported. Hovland, using [[experimental psychology]], found significant effects of information on longer-term behaviour and attitudes, particularly in areas where most people have little direct experience (eg politics) and have a high degree of trust in the source (eg broadcasting). It should be noted that since class has become an increasingly less good indicator of party (since the surveys of the 40s and 50s) the [[Swing vote|floating voter]] today is no longer the apathetic voter, but likely to be more well-informed than the consistent voter - and this mainly through the media.
 
There are also empirical problems with many of these early surveys, with researchers often ignoring important findings which would ascribe significant influence to the media (eg Lazarsfeld, Berelson and Gaudet note in ''The People’s Choice'' that 58% of voting changes were made without any remembered personal contact and were very often dependent on the mass media - changes being widely distributed among those who changed their opinion. But this effect was ignored in their conclusion of little direct media influence). Other studies supporting the opinion leader theory failed to distinguish between opinion leading in consumer and political behaviour. In political behaviour opinion leading tends to correlate positively with status, whereas this is not the case in consumer behaviour (breakfast cereals etc). So for political behaviour, the general conclusion that the media merely fixes (confirms) people’s opinion is not supported. Hovland, using [[experimental psychology]], found significant effects of information on longer-term behaviour and attitudes, particularly in areas where most people have little direct experience (eg politics) and have a high degree of trust in the source (eg broadcasting). It should be noted that since class has become an increasingly less good indicator of party (since the surveys of the 40s and 50s) the [[Swing vote|floating voter]] today is no longer the apathetic voter, but likely to be more well-informed than the consistent voter - and this mainly through the media.
  
==Theories==
 
===Habermas===
 
In historical terms, as Thompson points out, the development of communications and [[transport]] is one of the driving forces behind the development of modern society.<ref>Thompson, J. (1995), ''The Media and Modernity.'' Stanford University Press. ISBN 0804726795</ref> It made possible the [[industrial revolution]] and continues to be essential to the coherence of modern society. For [[Jürgen Habermas]], the development of mass media was a crucial factor in the transition from an absolutist regime to liberal-democratic society. He develops the notion that society became increasingly polarised into the spheres of 'public authority' on the one hand (referring to the emergence of the state and associated political activity); and the 'private' sphere on the other (which was the intimate domain of private relationships and the family). With the invention of the [[printing press]] and the subsequent availability of [[newspapers]] and various other forms of printed literature, however, Habermas sees  the emergence of an intermediate sphere which he refers to as the ‘bourgeois [[public sphere]]’. Here, individuals gather together to critically discuss and evaluate contemporary issues, stimulated by the contents of the open press, in a fashion reminiscent of the Greek [[agora]]. Habermas claims that this public use of reason not only acts as a regulatory mechanism over the [[state]], which is now highly visible, but also as a catalyst for the replacement of the absolutist regime with a liberal democratic [[government]].
 
  
However, this sphere of public discourse is transient, and will eventually disappear as increasing state intervention blurs the boundaries between public and private. At the same time, commercialisation of the media will radically alter its characteristics, as it becomes merely a tool for political manipulation, largely dependent on satisfying advertisers, readers and information sources such as the government. This can easily lead to a chase towards the lowest common denominator. This can be justified on the grounds of the massive widening of audience compared to the pre-industrial press, but it must be remembered that what is being conveyed to the masses is radically different from what was newsworthy then. Mass media today is about [[culture]] - but a culture selected for representation by the media. This process of the ‘refeudalisation of the public sphere’ will leave the public exempt from political discussions. It could be argued that a new kind of absolutism emerges as a result of an abuse of [[democracy]].
 
  
===Frankfurt School===   
+
==Opinion Polls==
Habermas depends to some extent on some early critiques of the media from the ‘[[Frankfurt School]]’, such as that of [[Max Horkheimer]], [[Theodor Adorno]] and [[Herbert Marcuse]], for whom the media was a 'culture industry' impacting on a sea of passive individuals, who merely absorb any information they are exposed to. (There is an influence from [[Karl Marx]] here, with links to the theory of [[alienation]].) The cause of this is the commodification of art and culture, which allows the possibility of "manipulation by demagogues" (Thompson, 1995). The Frankfurt School, which arose as an attempt to explain the success of [[Nazism]] in Weimar Germany, sees the loss of individuality through decline of [[privacy]] as the cause of dependence on great mass organisations. The interdependence of highly specialised individuals, or what [[Émile Durkheim]] called ‘[[organic solidarity]]’, is seen as being succeeded by a new and barbarous homogeneity. Only a ‘mechanical’ cohesion is possible, dependent on similarity and standardisation. Horkheimer argued that, paradoxically, individuality was impaired by the decline in the impulse for [[collective action]]. ‘As the ordinary man withdraws from participating in political affairs, society tends to revert to the law of the jungle, which crushes all vestiges of individuality.’  In this analysis the Frankfurters saw [[totalitarianism]] emerging as a result of corrupt social institutions and the decline of liberal principles. Thus Horkheimer: “Just as the slogans of rugged individualism are politically useful to large trusts in society seeking exemption from social control, so in mass culture the rhetoric of individuality, by imposing patterns for collective imitation, subverts the very principle to which it gives lip service.” Adorno in ''The Jargon of Authenticity'' notes that “mass media can create an aura which makes the spectator seem to experience a non-existent actuality”. Thus a mass-produced, artificial culture replaces what went before.
 
  
As in [[Jerry Mander]]’s work (see below), atomised individuals of mass society lose their souls to the phantom delights of the [[film]], the [[soap opera]], and the [[variety show]]. They fall into a stupor; an apathetic hypnosis Lazarsfeld was to call the ‘narcotizing dysfunction’ of exposure to mass media. Individuals become ‘irrational victims of false wants’ - the wants which corporations have thrust upon them, and continue to thrust upon them, through both the advertising in the media (with its continual exhortation to consume) and through the individualist consumption culture it promulgates. Marcuse describes this as a process where addiction to media leads to absolute docility, and the public becomes ‘enchanted and transformed into a clientele by the suppliers of popular culture.’ [[David Riesman]] in ''[[The Lonely Crowd]]'' claims that “Glamour in politics, the packaging of the leader, the treatment of events by the mass media, substitutes for the self-interest of the inner directed man the abandonment to society of the outer directed man.” In other words, the creation of the public sphere implies a fundamental change in social relations and individuals’ ability to model their self-image on some projected normality.
+
Opinion polls are [[statistical survey|surveys]] of [[opinion]] using [[sampling (statistics)|sampling]]. They are usually designed to represent the opinions of a population by asking a small number of people a series of questions and then extrapolating the answers to the larger group.
 
 
Thus, according to the Frankfurt School, [[leisure]] has been industrialised. The production of culture had become standardised and dominated by the profit motive as in other industries. In a mass society leisure is constantly used to induce the appropriate values and motives in the public. The modern media train the young for consumption. ‘Leisure had ceased to be the opposite of work, and had become a preparation for it.’ Marcuse points out the ‘Bach in the kitchen’ phenomenon: the fact that modern methods of reproduction have increased the quantity of music, art, and literature available to the public does not mean that culture spreads to the masses; rather that culture is destroyed in order to make entertainment. ‘At its worst mass culture threatens not merely to cretinise our taste,’ argues Rosenberg, ‘but to brutalise our senses while paving the way to totalitarianism.’ Lazarsfeld and Merton put the case succinctly: ‘Economic power seems to have reduced direct exploitation and to have turned to a subtler type of psychological exploitation,’ they wrote of the US in the 50s. Overt totalitarian force was increasingly obsolescent. Radio, film and television seemed even more effective than terror in producing compliance.
 
 
 
Marcuse notes a key part of this process is its sheer, relentless omnipresence: “The preconditioning does not start with the mass production of radio or TV [at a given point in time]. The people enter this stage as preconditioned receptacles of long standing. In this more complex view the public do no abdicate rational consideration of their interest blindly. More subtly, the whole basis of rational calculation is undermined.”
 
 
 
==Measuring public opinion==
 
 
 
'''Opinion polls''' are [[statistical survey|surveys]] of [[opinion]] using [[sampling (statistics)|sampling]]. They are usually designed to represent the opinions of a population by asking a small number of people a series of questions and then extrapolating the answers to the larger group.
 
  
 
===History of Opinion Polls===
 
===History of Opinion Polls===
  
The first known example of an opinion poll was a local straw vote conducted by The Harrisburg Pennsylvanian in [[1824]], showing [[Andrew Jackson]] leading [[John Quincy Adams]] by 335 votes to 169 in the contest for the [[President of the United States|United States Presidency]]. Such straw votes&mdash;unweighted and unscientific&mdash; gradually became more popular; but they remained local, usually city-wide phenomena. In [[1916]], the ''[[Literary Digest]]'' embarked on a national survey (partly as a circulation-raising exercise) and correctly predicted [[Woodrow Wilson]]'s election as President. Mailing out millions of [[postcard]]s and simply counting the returns, the ''Digest'' correctly called the following four presidential elections.
+
The first known example of an opinion poll was a local straw vote conducted by The Harrisburg Pennsylvanian in 1824, showing [[Andrew Jackson]] leading [[John Quincy Adams]] by 335 votes to 169 in the contest for the [[President of the United States|United States Presidency]]. Such straw votes&mdash;unweighted and unscientific&mdash; gradually became more popular; but they remained local, usually city-wide phenomena. In 1916, the ''[[Literary Digest]]'' embarked on a national survey (partly as a circulation-raising exercise) and correctly predicted [[Woodrow Wilson]]'s election as President. Mailing out millions of [[postcard]]s and simply counting the returns, the ''Digest'' correctly called the following four presidential elections.
  
 
In 1936, however, the ''Digest'' came unstuck. Its 2.3 million "voters" constituted a huge sample; however they were generally more affluent Americans who tended to have [[United States Republican Party|Republican]] sympathies. The ''Literary Digest'' did nothing to correct this bias. The week before election day, it reported that [[Alf Landon]] was far more popular than [[Franklin Delano Roosevelt|Franklin D. Roosevelt]]. At the same time, [[George Gallup]] conducted a far smaller, but more scientifically-based survey, in which he polled a demographically representative sample. Gallup correctly predicted Roosevelt's landslide victory. The ''Literary Digest'' went out of business soon afterwards, while the polling industry started to take off .
 
In 1936, however, the ''Digest'' came unstuck. Its 2.3 million "voters" constituted a huge sample; however they were generally more affluent Americans who tended to have [[United States Republican Party|Republican]] sympathies. The ''Literary Digest'' did nothing to correct this bias. The week before election day, it reported that [[Alf Landon]] was far more popular than [[Franklin Delano Roosevelt|Franklin D. Roosevelt]]. At the same time, [[George Gallup]] conducted a far smaller, but more scientifically-based survey, in which he polled a demographically representative sample. Gallup correctly predicted Roosevelt's landslide victory. The ''Literary Digest'' went out of business soon afterwards, while the polling industry started to take off .
Line 69: Line 61:
  
 
===Potential for inaccuracy===
 
===Potential for inaccuracy===
====Sampling error====
+
There exist a number of potential inaccuracies when relying on opinion polls. These include sampling errors, nonresponse bias, response bias, poor wording of questions, and coverage bias.
 
 
All polls based on  samples are subject to [[sampling error]] which reflects the effects of chance in the sampling process. The uncertainty is often expressed as a [[margin of error]]. The margin of error does not reflect other sources of error, such as measurement error.  A poll with a random sample of 1,000 people has margin of sampling error of 3% for the estimated percentage of the whole population. A 3% margin of error means that 95% of the time the procedure used would give an estimate within 3% of the percentage to be estimated. The margin of error can be reduced by using a larger sample, however if a pollster wishes to reduce the margin of error to 1% they would need a sample of around 10,000 people. In practice pollsters need to balance the cost of a large sample against the reduction in sampling error and a sample size of around 500-1,000 is a typical compromise for political polls. (Note that to get 500 complete responses it may be necessary to make thousands of phone calls.)[http://www.publicagenda.org/polling/polling_error.cfm]
 
 
 
====Nonresponse bias====
 
 
 
Since some people do not answer calls from strangers, or refuse to answer the poll, poll samples may not be representative samples from a population. Because of this [[selection bias]], the characteristics of those who agree to be interviewed may be markedly different from those who decline.  That is, the actual sample is a biased version of the universe the pollster wants to analyze. In these cases, bias introduces new errors, one way or the other, that are in addition to errors caused by sample size. Error due to bias does not become smaller with larger sample sizes.  If the people who refuse to answer, or are never reached, have the same characteristics as the people who do answer, the final results will be unbiased.  If the people who do not answer have different opinions then there is bias in the results.  In terms of election polls, studies suggest that bias effects are small, but each polling firm has its own formulas on how to adjust weights to minimize selection bias.[http://abcnews.go.com/images/pdf/responserates.pdf]
 
 
 
====Response bias====
 
 
 
Survey results may be affected by [[response bias]], where the answers given by respondents do not reflect their true beliefs. This may be deliberately engineered by unscrupulous pollsters in a [[push poll]], but more often is a result of the detailed wording or ordering of questions (see below). Respondents may deliberately try to manipulate the outcome of a poll by e.g. advocating a more extreme position than they actually hold in order to boost their side of the argument or give rapid and ill-considered answers in order to hasten the end of their questioning. Respondents may also feel under social pressure not to give an unpopular answer. An example of this involves minority groups as mentioned in the 2006 Tennessee senatorial race. Polling numbers were somewhat skewed by the Black candidate tax. If the results of surveys are widely publicised this effect may be magnified - the so-called [[spiral of silence]].
 
 
 
====Wording of questions====
 
 
 
It is well established that the wording of the questions, the order in which they are asked and the number and form of alternative answers offered can influence results of polls. Thus comparisons between polls often boil down to the wording of the question. On some issues, question wording can result in quite pronounced differences between surveys. [http://www.publicagenda.org/issues/red_flags.cfm?issue_type=higher_education#affirmative_action][http://www.publicagenda.org/issues/red_flags.cfm?issue_type=gay_rights#benefits][http://www.publicagenda.org/issues/red_flags.cfm?issue_type=abortion#mixed] This can also, however, be a result of legitimately conflicted feelings or evolving attitudes, rather than a poorly constructed survey.[http://www.publicagenda.org/polling/polling_stages.cfm] One way in which pollsters attempt to minimize this effect is to ask the same set of questions over time, in order to track changes in opinion. Another common technique is to rotate the order in which questions are asked. Many pollsters also [[split-sample]]. This involves having two different versions of a question, with each version presented to half the respondents.
 
 
 
The most effective controls, used by [[attitude (psychology)|attitude]] researchers, are:
 
 
 
* asking enough questions to allow all aspects of an issue to be covered and to control effects due to the form of the question (such as positive or negative wording), the adequacy of the number being established quantitatively with [[psychometrics|psychometric]] measures such as reliability coefficients, and
 
 
 
* analyzing the results with psychometric techniques which synthesize the answers into a few reliable scores and detect ineffective questions.
 
 
 
These controls are not widely used in the polling industry.
 
 
 
====Coverage bias====
 
  
Another source of error is the use of samples that are not representative of the population as a consequence of the methodology used, as was the experience of the ''Literary Digest'' in 1936. For example, telephone sampling has a built-in error because in many times and places, those with telephones have generally been richer than those withoutAlternately, in some places, many people have only [[mobile telephone]]s. Because pollers cannot call mobile phones (it is unlawful to make unsolicited calls to phones where the phone's owner may be charged simply for taking a call), these individuals will never be included in the polling sample. If the subset of the population without cell phones differs markedly from the rest of the population, these differences can skew the results of the poll. Polling organizations have developed many weighting techniques to help overcome these deficiencies, to varying degrees of success. Several studies of mobile phone users by the Pew Research Center in the U.S. concluded that the absence of mobile users was not unduly skewing results, at least not yet. [http://pewresearch.org/obdeck/?ObDeckID=80]
+
[[Sampling error]] reflects the effects of chance in the sampling process. The uncertainty is often expressed as a [[margin of error]].  The margin of error does not reflect other sources of error, such as measurement error.  A poll with a random sample of 1,000 people has margin of sampling error of 3% for the estimated percentage of the whole population. A 3% margin of error means that 95% of the time the procedure used would give an estimate within 3% of the percentage to be estimated. The margin of error can be reduced by using a larger sample, however if a pollster wishes to reduce the margin of error to 1% they would need a sample of around 10,000 people.
  
An oft-quoted example of opinion polls succumbing to errors was the [[UK general election, 1992|UK General Election]] of 1992. Despite the polling organisations using different methodologies virtually all the polls in the lead up to the vote (and [[exit poll]]s taken on voting day) showed a lead for the opposition Labour party but the actual vote gave a clear victory to the ruling Conservative party.
+
Nonresponse bias occurs because some people do not answer calls from strangers, or refuse to answer the poll, so poll samples may not be representative samples from a population. Because of this [[selection bias]], the characteristics of those who agree to be interviewed may be markedly different from those who decline. If the people who do not answer have different opinions then there is bias in the results.  Response bias occurs when respondents deliberately try to manipulate the outcome of a poll by e.g. advocating a more extreme position than they actually hold in order to boost their side of the argument or give rapid and ill-considered answers in order to hasten the end of their questioning. Respondents may also feel under social pressure not to give an unpopular answer.
  
In their deliberations after this embarrassment the pollsters advanced several ideas to account for their errors, including:
+
It is well established that the wording of the questions, the order in which they are asked and the number and form of alternative answers offered can influence results of polls. Thus comparisons between polls often boil down to the wording of the question. On some issues, question wording can result in quite pronounced differences between surveys. One way in which pollsters attempt to minimize this effect is to ask the same set of questions over time, in order to track changes in opinion. Another common technique is to rotate the order in which questions are asked. Many pollsters also [[split-sample]]. This involves having two different versions of a question, with each version presented to half the respondents.
  
* '''Late [[swing (politics)|swing]]'''. The Conservatives gained from people who switched to them at the last minute, so the error was not as great as it first appeared.
+
Another source of error is the use of samples that are not representative of the population as a consequence of the methodology used, known as coverage bias. For example, telephone sampling has a built-in error because in many times and places, those with telephones have generally been richer than those withoutAlternately, in some places, many people have only [[mobile telephone]]s. Because pollers cannot call mobile phones (it is unlawful to make unsolicited calls to phones where the phone's owner may be charged simply for taking a call), these individuals will never be included in the polling sample. If the subset of the population without cell phones differs markedly from the rest of the population, these differences can skew the results of the poll. Polling organizations have developed many weighting techniques to help overcome these deficiencies, to varying degrees of success. Several studies of mobile phone users by the Pew Research Center in the U.S. concluded that the absence of mobile users was not unduly skewing results, at least not yet.<ref>[http://pewresearch.org/obdeck/?ObDeckID=80 Cell-Only Voters Not Very Different] Pew Research Center. Retrieved March 12, 2007.</ref>
 
 
* '''Nonresponse bias'''. Conservative voters were less likely to  participate in the survey than in the past and were thus underrepresented.
 
 
 
* The '''[[spiral of silence]]'''. The Conservatives had suffered a sustained period of unpopularity as a result of economic recession and a series of minor scandals. Some Conservative supporters felt under pressure to give a more popular answer.
 
 
 
The relative importance of these factors was, and remains, a matter of controversy, but since then the polling organisations have adjusted their methodologies and have achieved more accurate predictions in subsequent elections.
 
 
 
===Polling organizations===
 
 
 
There are many polling organizationsThe most famous is the [[Gallup poll]], created by [[George Gallup]].
 
 
 
Other major polling organizations in the United States include:
 
 
 
*[[Quinnipiac Polls]], run by Quinnipiac University in Hamden, Connecticut, and started as a student project.
 
*The [[Pew Research Center]] conducts polls concentrating on media and political beliefs.
 
*The [[Harris Poll]].
 
*The [[Roper Poll]].
 
*[[Nielsen Ratings]], virtually always for television.
 
*[[Zogby]] International has been tracking public opinion since 1984, the nation's first internet survey organization, focused mainly on politics.
 
*[[Rasmussen Reports]], public opinion, focused mainly on politics.
 
*[[Greenberg Quinlan Rosner]]  (Democratic)
 
*[[Public Opinion Strategies]]  (Republican)
 
*The [[National Opinion Research Center]].
 
*[[Public Agenda]], conducts research bridging the gap between what American leaders think and what the public really thinks.
 
 
 
In the [[United Kingdom]], the most notable "pollsters" are:
 
*[[MORI]]. This polling organisation is notable for only selecting those who say that they are "likely" to vote. This has tended to favour the [[Conservative Party (UK)|Conservative Party]] in recent years.
 
*[[YouGov]], an online pollster.
 
*[[GfK NOP]]
 
*[[ICR/International Communications Research|ICR]]
 
*[[ICM (polling)|ICM]]
 
*Populus, official [[The Times]] pollster.
 
 
 
In [[Australia]] the most notable companies are:
 
*[[Newspoll]]
 
*[[Roy Morgan Research]]
 
 
 
In [[Canada]] the most notable companies are:
 
*[[Ipsos-Reid]]
 
*Environics
 
*Ekos
 
*Decima
 
*Leger
 
*CROP
 
 
 
All the major [[television network]]s, alone or in conjunction with the largest [[newspaper]]s or [[magazine]]s, in virtually every country with elections, operate polling operations, alone or in groups.  
 
 
 
Several organizations monitor the behaviour of pollsters and the use of polling data, including PEW and, in Canada, the Laurier Institute for the Study of Public Opinion and Policy.[http://www.wlu.ca/lispop/lispop]
 
 
 
The best-known failure of opinion polling to date in the [[United States]] was the prediction that [[Thomas Dewey]] would defeat [[Harry S. Truman]] in the [[U.S. presidential election, 1948|1948 U.S. Presidential election]].  Major polling organizations, including Gallup and Roper, indicated a landslide victory for Dewey.
 
 
 
In the United Kingdom, most polls failed to predict the Conservative election victories of [[1970]] and [[1992]], and Labour's victory in [[1974]]. However, their figures at other elections have been generally accurate.
 
 
 
==The influence of opinion polls==
 
  
 +
===The influence of opinion polls===
 
By providing information about voting intentions, opinion polls can sometimes influence the behaviour of electors. The various theories about how this happens can be split up into two groups: bandwagon/underdog effects, and strategic ('tactical') voting.
 
By providing information about voting intentions, opinion polls can sometimes influence the behaviour of electors. The various theories about how this happens can be split up into two groups: bandwagon/underdog effects, and strategic ('tactical') voting.
  
A [[Bandwagon effect]] occurs when the poll prompts voters to back the candidate shown to be winning in the poll. The idea that voters are susceptible to such effects is old, stemming at least from 1884; Safire (1993: 43) reported that it was first used in a political cartoon in the magazine Puck in that year. It has also remained persistent in spite of a lack of empirical corroberation until the late 20th century. [[George Gallup]] spent much effort in vain trying to discredit this theory in his time by presenting empirical research. A recent meta-study of scientific research on this topic indicates that from the 1980's onward the Bandwagon effect is found more often by researchers (Irwin & van Holsteyn 2000).
+
A [[Bandwagon effect]] occurs when the poll prompts voters to back the candidate shown to be winning in the poll. The idea that voters are susceptible to such effects is old, stemming at least from 1884; William Safire reported that it was first used in a political cartoon in the magazine Puck in that year. It has also remained persistent in spite of a lack of empirical corroberation until the late 20th century. [[George Gallup]] spent much effort in vain trying to discredit this theory in his time by presenting empirical research.<ref>[http://www.science-spirit.org/webexclusives.php?article_id=675 The Surveyor] Science & Spirit. Retrieved March 12, 2007.</ref> A recent meta-study of scientific research on this topic indicates that from the 1980's onward the Bandwagon effect is found more often by researchers.
  
The opposite of the bandwagon effect is the [[Underdog effect]]. It is often mentioned in the media. This occurs when people vote, out of sympathy, for the party perceived to be 'losing' the elections. There is less empirical evidence for the existence of this effect than there is for the existence of the Bandwagon effect (Irwin & van Holsteyn 2000).  
+
The opposite of the bandwagon effect is the [[Underdog effect]]. It is often mentioned in the media. This occurs when people vote, out of sympathy, for the party perceived to be 'losing' the elections. There is less empirical evidence for the existence of this effect than there is for the existence of the Bandwagon effect.
  
 
The second category of theories on how polls directly affect voting is called strategic or [[tactical voting]]. This theory is based on the idea that voters view the act of voting as a means of selecting a government. Thus they will sometimes not choose the candidate they prefer on ground of ideology or sympathy, but another, less-preferred, candidate from strategic considerations. An example can be found in the [[United Kingdom general election, 1997]]. Then Cabinet Minister, [[Michael Portillo]]'s constituency of [[Enfield]] was believed to be a [[safe seat]] but opinion polls showed the [[Labour Party (UK)|Labour]] candidate [[Stephen Twigg]] steadily gaining support, which may have prompted undecided voters or supporters of other parties to support Twigg in order to remove Portillo. Another example is the Boomerang effect where the likely supporters of the candidate shown to be winning feel that s/he is "home and dry" and that their vote is not required, thus allowing another candidate to win.
 
The second category of theories on how polls directly affect voting is called strategic or [[tactical voting]]. This theory is based on the idea that voters view the act of voting as a means of selecting a government. Thus they will sometimes not choose the candidate they prefer on ground of ideology or sympathy, but another, less-preferred, candidate from strategic considerations. An example can be found in the [[United Kingdom general election, 1997]]. Then Cabinet Minister, [[Michael Portillo]]'s constituency of [[Enfield]] was believed to be a [[safe seat]] but opinion polls showed the [[Labour Party (UK)|Labour]] candidate [[Stephen Twigg]] steadily gaining support, which may have prompted undecided voters or supporters of other parties to support Twigg in order to remove Portillo. Another example is the Boomerang effect where the likely supporters of the candidate shown to be winning feel that s/he is "home and dry" and that their vote is not required, thus allowing another candidate to win.
 
These effects only indicate how opinion polls ''directly'' affect political choices of the electorate. Other effect can be found on journalists, politicians, political parties, civil servants etc. in, among other things, the form of media [[Framing (communication theory)]] and party ideology shifts.
 
  
 
==Public Opinion in the Internet Age==
 
==Public Opinion in the Internet Age==

Revision as of 14:38, 12 March 2007


Public opinion is the aggregate of individual attitudes or beliefs held by the adult population. Public opinion can be influenced by public relations and the political media. Additionally, mass media utilizes a wide variety of advertising techniques to get their message out and change the minds of people. A continuously used technique is propaganda. The tide of public opinion becomes more and more crucial during political elections, most importantly elections determining the national executive.

Public opinion is frequently measured using the method of survey sampling.

History

Public opinion developed as a concept with the rise of a 'public' in the eighteenth century. The English term ‘public opinion’ dates from the eighteenth century and derives from the French ‘l’opinion publique’, first used by Montaigne two centuries earlier in 1588. This came about through urbanization and other political and social forces. It became important what people thought as forms of political contention changed.

Adam Smith refered to it in his Theory of Moral Sentiments, but Jeremy Bentham was the first British writer to fully develop theories of public opinion. He reasoned that public opinion had the power to ensure that rulers would rule for the greatest happiness of the greater number.

Using the conceptional tools of his theory of Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft, the German sociologist Ferdinand Tönnies argued that 'public opinion' has the equivalent social functions in societies (Gesellschaften) which religion has in communities (Gemeinschaften).[1]

The German philosopher Jürgen Habermas contributed the idea of "Public Sphere" to the discussion of public opinion. Public Sphere, as he argued, is where “something approaching public opinion can be formed.”[2] It is featured as universal access, rational debate, and disregard for rank. However, these three features for how public opinion should be formed are not in place in western democracy. Public opinion is highly susceptible to elite manipulation.

American sociolologist Herbert Blumer proposed a somewhat different conception of the "public," as a form of collective behavior (another specialized term) which is made up of those who are discussing a given public issue at any one time. Given this definition, there are many publics; each of them comes into being when an issue arises and ceases to exist when the issue is resolved. Blumer claimed that since people participate in a public to different degrees, public opinion polling cannot measure the public: An archbishop's participation is more important than that of a bum. The "mass," in which people independently make decisions about, for example, which brand of toothpaste to buy, is a form of collective behavior different from the public.

Formation of Public Opinion

Public opinion is a strange, fickle creature. Many things influence the constitution of public thought, sometimes seemingly at random. The mass media, word of mouth, economy, sense of community, advertising, and propaganda all have some effect on public opinion. Paul Lazarsfeld argued that the public forms its opinion in a two-stage process. He thought most people rely on opinion leaders. These opinion leaders are affected by world events and then pass opinions down to less active members of society. There are other contributors to the shaping of public opinion, however.

Lazarsfeld believed that the mass media was the main source of information for opinion leaders, but his theory may have missed the tremendous impact the mass media has over every citizen, not just a select few. Most people gather all of their information regarding current events from some outlet of the mass media be it large newspapers, television news, or the internet. The information these people retain is largely colored by the opinions of those presenting them. As a result, many people take on the opinions of their news presenters (although one could also argue that they gravitate to those broadcast outlets because of similar shared opinions).

Mass media and public opinion

The mass media plays a crucial role in forming and reflecting public opinion: it communicates the world to individuals, and it reproduces modern society's self-image. Critiques in the early-to-mid twentieth century suggested that the media destroys the individual's capacity to act autonomously - sometimes being ascribed an influence reminiscent of the telescreens of the dystopian novel 1984. Later empirical studies, however, suggest a more complex interaction between the media and society, with individuals actively interpreting and evaluating the media and the information it provides. In the twenty-first century, with the rise of the internet, the two-way relationship between mass media and public opinion is beginning to change, with the advent of new technologies such as blogging.

Ownership of Media and the Crafting of Opinion

The long-term consequences of this are significant in conjunction with the continuing concentration of ownership and control of the media, leading to accusations of a 'media elite' having a form of 'cultural dictatorship'. Thus the continuing debate about the influence of 'media barons' such as Conrad Black and Rupert Murdoch. For example, the Guardian reported the Murdoch-owned HarperCollins' refusal to publish Chris Patten's East and West, because of the former Hong Kong Governor's description of the Chinese leadership as "faceless Stalinists" possibly being damaging to Murdoch's Chinese broadcasting interests.[3] In this case, the author was able to have the book accepted by another publisher, but this kind of censorship may point the way to the future. A related, but more insidious, form is that of self-censorship by members of the media in the interests of the owner, in the interests of their careers.

The agenda-setting process is partly one which is an almost unavoidable function of the bureaucratic process involved in newsgathering by the large organisations which make up much of the mass media. (Just four main news agencies - AP, UPI, Reuters and Agence-France-Presse - claim together to provide 90% of the total news output of the world’s press, radio and television.) For example, in order to get into the news, events have to happen in places convenient for the newsgathering agencies, come from a reliable and predictable source, and fit into journalists’ framework of news values:

[J]ournalists, who are better seen as bureaucrats than as buccaneers, begin their work from a stock of plausible, well-defined and largely unconscious assumptions. Part of their job is to translate untidy reality into neat stories with beginnings, middles and denouements. … The values which inform the selection of news items usually reinforce conventional opinions and established authority. At the same time, a process of simplification filters out the disturbing or the unexpected. The need of the media to secure instant attention creates a strong prejudice in favour of familiar stories and themes, and a slowness of response when reality breaks the conventions.[4]

The effects of the mass media on public opinion relate not merely to the way newsworthy events are perceived (and which are reported at all), but also to a multitude of cultural influences which operate through the mass media. Thus Lang and Lang claim that "The mass media force attention to certain issues. They build up public images of political figures. They are constantly presenting objects suggesting what individuals in the mass should think about, know about, have feelings about."[5]

Stuart Hall points out that because some of the media produce material which often is good, impartial, and serious, they are accorded a high degree of respect and authority. But in practice the ethic of the press and television is closely related to that of the homogeneous establishment, providing a vital support for the existing order. But independence (eg of the BBC) is not “a mere cover, it is central to the way power and ideology are mediated in societies like ours.” The public are bribed with good radio, television and newspapers into an acceptance of the biased, the misleading, and the status quo. The media are not, according to this approach, crude agents of propaganda. They organise public understanding. However, the overall interpretations they provide in the long run are those which are most preferred by, and least challenging to, those with economic power.

Mass Media and Political Opinion

In other words, political advertising impacts not on blank-sheet individuals but on people with existing beliefs formed over long periods of time, which they are correspondingly reluctant to change. Moreover, the people who are most exposed to the media are those who know from the outset whom they will vote for, and are therefore least likely to be influenced by propaganda. Thus it appears that the notion that the people who switch parties during the campaign are mainly the reasoned, thoughtful people convinced by the issues, is completely unfounded. Lazarsfeld et al claim the real influence on undecided voters is the 'opinion leader', the individual whose own vote intention is secure, and who is well informed on the issues. Thus personal influence is primarily of greater importance than media influence albeit using information initially acquired through the media. This may have something to do with trust and authority: both opinion leaders and the general public will select the evidence and information which supports their view, placing greater weight on more trustworthy sources. For the opinion-leader theory to be true, then, the general public would have to place greater trust in opinion leaders than in the media, so that the opinion leaders act as mediators between the public and the media, personalising and making authoritative the information the media provides. Thus "...the person-to-person influence reaches the ones who are more susceptible to change and serves as a bridge over which formal media of communications extend their influence."[6] From a psychological viewpoint, we may understand the personal influence of the opinion leaders in terms of group association: perceived as representing the group's desirable characteristics, other group members will aspire to the leaders’ viewpoints in order to maintain group cohesiveness and thus indirectly self-assurance. However, the separation of group leaders from the general public is arguably an over-simplification of the process of media influences.

There are also empirical problems with many of these early surveys, with researchers often ignoring important findings which would ascribe significant influence to the media (eg Lazarsfeld, Berelson and Gaudet note in The People’s Choice that 58% of voting changes were made without any remembered personal contact and were very often dependent on the mass media - changes being widely distributed among those who changed their opinion. But this effect was ignored in their conclusion of little direct media influence). Other studies supporting the opinion leader theory failed to distinguish between opinion leading in consumer and political behaviour. In political behaviour opinion leading tends to correlate positively with status, whereas this is not the case in consumer behaviour (breakfast cereals etc). So for political behaviour, the general conclusion that the media merely fixes (confirms) people’s opinion is not supported. Hovland, using experimental psychology, found significant effects of information on longer-term behaviour and attitudes, particularly in areas where most people have little direct experience (eg politics) and have a high degree of trust in the source (eg broadcasting). It should be noted that since class has become an increasingly less good indicator of party (since the surveys of the 40s and 50s) the floating voter today is no longer the apathetic voter, but likely to be more well-informed than the consistent voter - and this mainly through the media.


Opinion Polls

Opinion polls are surveys of opinion using sampling. They are usually designed to represent the opinions of a population by asking a small number of people a series of questions and then extrapolating the answers to the larger group.

History of Opinion Polls

The first known example of an opinion poll was a local straw vote conducted by The Harrisburg Pennsylvanian in 1824, showing Andrew Jackson leading John Quincy Adams by 335 votes to 169 in the contest for the United States Presidency. Such straw votes—unweighted and unscientific— gradually became more popular; but they remained local, usually city-wide phenomena. In 1916, the Literary Digest embarked on a national survey (partly as a circulation-raising exercise) and correctly predicted Woodrow Wilson's election as President. Mailing out millions of postcards and simply counting the returns, the Digest correctly called the following four presidential elections.

In 1936, however, the Digest came unstuck. Its 2.3 million "voters" constituted a huge sample; however they were generally more affluent Americans who tended to have Republican sympathies. The Literary Digest did nothing to correct this bias. The week before election day, it reported that Alf Landon was far more popular than Franklin D. Roosevelt. At the same time, George Gallup conducted a far smaller, but more scientifically-based survey, in which he polled a demographically representative sample. Gallup correctly predicted Roosevelt's landslide victory. The Literary Digest went out of business soon afterwards, while the polling industry started to take off .

Gallup launched a subsidiary in the United Kingdom, where it correctly predicted Labour's victory in the 1945 general election, in contrast with virtually all other commentators, who expected the Conservative Party, led by Winston Churchill, to win easily.

By the 1950s, polling had spread to most democracies. Nowadays they reach virtually every country, although in more autocratic societies they tend to avoid sensitive political topics. In Iraq, surveys conducted soon after the 2003 war helped to measure the true feelings of Iraqi citizens to Saddam Hussein, post-war conditions and the presence of US forces.

For many years, opinion polls were conducted mainly face-to-face, either in the street or in people's homes. This method remains widely used, but in some countries it has been overtaken by telephone polls, which can be conducted faster and more cheaply. Because of the common practice of telemarketers to sell products under the guise of a telephone survey and due to the proliferation of residential call screening devices and use of cell phones, response rates for phone surveys have been plummeting. Mailed surveys have become the data collection method of choice among local governments that conduct a citizen survey to track service quality and manage resource allocation. In recent years, Internet and short message service (SMS, or text) surveys have become increasingly popular, but most of these draw on whomever wishes to participate rather than a scientific sample of the population, and are therefore not generally considered accurate.

Potential for inaccuracy

There exist a number of potential inaccuracies when relying on opinion polls. These include sampling errors, nonresponse bias, response bias, poor wording of questions, and coverage bias.

Sampling error reflects the effects of chance in the sampling process. The uncertainty is often expressed as a margin of error. The margin of error does not reflect other sources of error, such as measurement error. A poll with a random sample of 1,000 people has margin of sampling error of 3% for the estimated percentage of the whole population. A 3% margin of error means that 95% of the time the procedure used would give an estimate within 3% of the percentage to be estimated. The margin of error can be reduced by using a larger sample, however if a pollster wishes to reduce the margin of error to 1% they would need a sample of around 10,000 people.

Nonresponse bias occurs because some people do not answer calls from strangers, or refuse to answer the poll, so poll samples may not be representative samples from a population. Because of this selection bias, the characteristics of those who agree to be interviewed may be markedly different from those who decline. If the people who do not answer have different opinions then there is bias in the results. Response bias occurs when respondents deliberately try to manipulate the outcome of a poll by e.g. advocating a more extreme position than they actually hold in order to boost their side of the argument or give rapid and ill-considered answers in order to hasten the end of their questioning. Respondents may also feel under social pressure not to give an unpopular answer.

It is well established that the wording of the questions, the order in which they are asked and the number and form of alternative answers offered can influence results of polls. Thus comparisons between polls often boil down to the wording of the question. On some issues, question wording can result in quite pronounced differences between surveys. One way in which pollsters attempt to minimize this effect is to ask the same set of questions over time, in order to track changes in opinion. Another common technique is to rotate the order in which questions are asked. Many pollsters also split-sample. This involves having two different versions of a question, with each version presented to half the respondents.

Another source of error is the use of samples that are not representative of the population as a consequence of the methodology used, known as coverage bias. For example, telephone sampling has a built-in error because in many times and places, those with telephones have generally been richer than those without. Alternately, in some places, many people have only mobile telephones. Because pollers cannot call mobile phones (it is unlawful to make unsolicited calls to phones where the phone's owner may be charged simply for taking a call), these individuals will never be included in the polling sample. If the subset of the population without cell phones differs markedly from the rest of the population, these differences can skew the results of the poll. Polling organizations have developed many weighting techniques to help overcome these deficiencies, to varying degrees of success. Several studies of mobile phone users by the Pew Research Center in the U.S. concluded that the absence of mobile users was not unduly skewing results, at least not yet.[7]

The influence of opinion polls

By providing information about voting intentions, opinion polls can sometimes influence the behaviour of electors. The various theories about how this happens can be split up into two groups: bandwagon/underdog effects, and strategic ('tactical') voting.

A Bandwagon effect occurs when the poll prompts voters to back the candidate shown to be winning in the poll. The idea that voters are susceptible to such effects is old, stemming at least from 1884; William Safire reported that it was first used in a political cartoon in the magazine Puck in that year. It has also remained persistent in spite of a lack of empirical corroberation until the late 20th century. George Gallup spent much effort in vain trying to discredit this theory in his time by presenting empirical research.[8] A recent meta-study of scientific research on this topic indicates that from the 1980's onward the Bandwagon effect is found more often by researchers.

The opposite of the bandwagon effect is the Underdog effect. It is often mentioned in the media. This occurs when people vote, out of sympathy, for the party perceived to be 'losing' the elections. There is less empirical evidence for the existence of this effect than there is for the existence of the Bandwagon effect.

The second category of theories on how polls directly affect voting is called strategic or tactical voting. This theory is based on the idea that voters view the act of voting as a means of selecting a government. Thus they will sometimes not choose the candidate they prefer on ground of ideology or sympathy, but another, less-preferred, candidate from strategic considerations. An example can be found in the United Kingdom general election, 1997. Then Cabinet Minister, Michael Portillo's constituency of Enfield was believed to be a safe seat but opinion polls showed the Labour candidate Stephen Twigg steadily gaining support, which may have prompted undecided voters or supporters of other parties to support Twigg in order to remove Portillo. Another example is the Boomerang effect where the likely supporters of the candidate shown to be winning feel that s/he is "home and dry" and that their vote is not required, thus allowing another candidate to win.

Public Opinion in the Internet Age

Jerry Mander, in Four Arguments for the Elimination of Television, takes a negative view on the current state of mass media affected public opinion. Mander argues that television has become the new transmission mechanism for cultural influences, but that because of the nature and structure of the medium, it encourages a global homogeneity of culture based on US cultural influences. He quotes as an example the introduction of television to the Northwest of Canada, populated mainly by Dene Indians and Inuit, which led to the erosion of traditional values, pastimes and occupations, and the desire of the young to learn English and acquire material possessions such as cars. The previous mode of cultural transmission - nightly story-telling - ended almost completely with the introduction of television, destroying “a bond of love and respect between the young and the old that was critical to the survival of native culture. Mander describes television as “the instrument for re-shaping our internal environments - our feelings, our thoughts, our ideas and our nervous systems - to match the re-created artificial environment that increasingly surrounds us: Commodity life; Technological passivity; Acceleration; Homogenisation.” (emphasis in original).

Mander’s theory is related to Jean Baudrillard’s concept of ‘hyperreality’. We can take the 1994 O.J. Simpson trial as an example, where the reality reported on was merely the catalyst for the simulacra (images) created, which defined the trial as a global event and made the trial more than it was. Essentially, hyperreality is the concept that the media is not merely a window on to the world (as if a visiting alien were watching TV), but is itself part of the reality it describes. Hence (although additionally there is the question of navel-gazing) the media’s obsession with media-created events.

For the future, the internet - through blogs, forums, wikis etc - may play a role in reclaiming the public sphere for liberal-democratic debate. The various means of communication available on the internet present the public with more outlets through which to express their opinions and for formerly marginalized groups of people to come together in central (virtual) locations, giving one voice to formerly disparate peoples. The internet offers newly focused discussion for these groups of people with the potential that their newfound single voices will be much louder in the public sphere. This could lead to the broaching of previously taboo or outlandish topics in mainstream culture and even the eventual shifting of that culture as a result.

Notes

  1. Tönnies, Ferdinand. Kritik der öffentlichen Meinung. 1922. Walter De Gruyter Inc. ISBN 3110153491
  2. Habermas, Jurgen. (1962), The Structural Transformation of the Public Sphere. Polity Press. ISBN 0745610773 p.351
  3. The Bookseller Guardian Unlimited. Retrieved February 13, 2007.
  4. Curran, James and Seaton, Jean. 2005. Power Without Responsibility. Taylor and Francis. ISBN 0415243890 ISBN 978-0415243896
  5. Lang K & Lang G.E. 1966. The Mass Media and Voting.
  6. Lazarsfeld, Berelson and Gaudet (1944), The People’s Choice
  7. Cell-Only Voters Not Very Different Pew Research Center. Retrieved March 12, 2007.
  8. The Surveyor Science & Spirit. Retrieved March 12, 2007.

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Adorno, Theodor (1973), The Jargon of Authenticity Routledge. ISBN 0415289912
  • Chomsky, Noam & Herman, Edward (1988, 2002). Manufacturing Consent: The Political Economy of the Mass Media. New York: Pantheon. ISBN 0375714499
  • Curran, J. & Seaton, J. (1988), Power without Responsibility. Taylor and Francis. ISBN 0415243890
  • Curran, J. & Gurevitch, M. (eds) (1991), Mass Media and Society Hodder Arnold. ISBN 0340884991
  • Habermas, J. (1962), The Structural Transformation of the Public Sphere Polity Press. ISBN 0745610773
  • Horkheimer (1947), The Eclipse of Reason, Oxford University Press ISBN 0826477933
  • Lang K & Lang G.E. (1966), The Mass Media and Voting
  • Lazarsfeld, Berelson and Gaudet (1944), The People’s Choice
  • Lippman, Walter. Public Opinion, 1921. Hard Press. ISBN 1406932949
  • Mander, Jerry, “The Tyranny of Television”, in Resurgence No. 165
  • McCombs, M & Shaw, D.L. (1972), 'The Agenda-setting Function of the Mass Media', Public Opinion Quarterly, 73, pp176-187
  • David Riesman (1950), The Lonely Crowd Yale University Press. ISBN 0300088655
  • Shamir, J. and Michal Shamir. The Anatomy of Public Opinion University of Michigan Press, 2000. ISBN 0472110225
  • Thompson, J. (1995), The Media and Modernity Stanford University Press. ISBN 0804726795
  • Trenaman J., and McQuail, D. (1961), Television and the Political Image. Methuen.

External links


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.