Difference between revisions of "Imprinting (psychology)" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
 
(21 intermediate revisions by 7 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
+
{{Ebcompleted}}{{Paid}}{{Approved}}{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}{{copyedited}}{{2Copyedited}}
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
{{Contracted}}
 
  
 +
'''Imprinting''' is used in [[psychology]] and [[ethology]] to describe any kind of [[learning]] that occurs at a particular age or stage of development. A phase-sensitive type of learning, it involves an organism recognizing the characteristics of certain stimuli that are subsequently "imprinted" onto the subject. Most occurrences involve learning to recognize one's parents or potential sexual partners, both of which have value for the survival of the species. Although most commonly observed in birds, this process has been observed in other species, leading to advances not only in the ability to assist various species but also in understanding how similar processes are involved in [[child development|human development]].
 +
{{toc}}
 +
Imprinting was initially researched in detail by [[Konrad Lorenz]], through his naturalistic studies of geese. The image of Lorenz followed by a family of goslings became familiar to many. Applications of this knowledge have been put to good use, ensuring that young raised in captivity learn survival skills, and, thus, can function in a relatively natural type of situation or even be re-introduced into the wild.
  
'''Imprinting''' is used in [[psychology]] and [[ethology]] to describe any kind of learning that occurrs at a particular age or stage of devlopement. Imprinting is a phase-sensitive type of learning that usually involves an organism recognizing the characteristics of certain stimuli that are subsequently "imprinted" onto the subject. Imprinting is an intriguing combination of both innate and environmental influences that shape and affect behavior.
+
==Outline of the theory==
 +
The most commonly found form of '''imprinting''' is known as "filial imprinting." Filial imprinting occurs when a young animal recognizes the characteristics of its parent. It is most obvious in nidifugous [[bird]]s (who leave the nest shortly after hatching) and [[reptile]]s, who imprint on their parents.   
  
==Outline of the Theory==
+
Filial imprinting was first reported in domestic [[chicken]]s, by the nineteenth century amateur [[biology|biologist]] [[Douglas Spalding]]. It was rediscovered by the early [[ethology|ethologist]] [[Oskar Heinroth]], and studied scientifically and popularized by his disciple, [[Konrad Lorenz]] in his work with graylag [[goose|geese]]. Lorenz demonstrated how [[incubator]]-hatched geese would imprint on the first suitable, moving stimulus they saw within what he called a "critical period" of about 36 hours shortly after hatching. Being present with the goslings during their hatching, Lorenz found that the goslings would imprint on himself. As a result of this, he is often depicted being followed by a gaggle of geese who had imprinted on him.
 
 
A widely researched form of imprinting is known as filial imprinting. Filial imprinting occurs when a young animal recognizes the characteristics of its parent.  It is most obvious in nidifugous birds and reptiles, who imprint on their parents. 
 
 
 
Filial imprinting was first reported in domestic chickens, by the 19th century amateur biologist Douglas Spalding. It was rediscovered by the early ethologist Oskar Heinroth, and studied scientifically and popularized by his disciple [[Konrad Lorenz]] working with greylag geese. Lorenz demonstrated how incubator-hatched geese would imprint on the first suitable moving stimulus they saw within what he called a "critical period" of about 36 hours shortly after hatching. Being present with the goslings during their hatching, Lorenz found that the goslings would imprint on himself. As a result of this he is often depicted being followed by a gaggle of geese who had imprinted on him
 
 
 
The Italian hang-glider pilot [[Angelo d'Arrigo]] manipulated Lorenz's technique to re-introduce endangered species. D'Arrigo noted that the flight of a non-motorised hang-glider is very similar to the flight patterns of migratory birds seeing as both use updrafts of thermal currents to gain altitude, permitting soaring flight over long distances. Birds hatched in captivity have no mentor birds to teach them their traditional migratory routes. The chicks hatched under the wing of his glider, and imprinted on him. Subsequently, he taught the fledglings to fly and to hunt. The young birds followed him not only on the ground (as with Lorenz) but also in the air as he took the path of various migratory routes. He flew across the [[Sahara]] and over the [[Mediterranean Sea]] to [[Sicily]] with [[eagle]]s, from [[Siberia]] to [[Iran]] with a flock of Siberian cranes, and over [[Everest]] with Nepalese eagles. In 2006, he worked with a condor in South America.
 
 
 
In project similar to d'Arrigo's efforts, orphaned Canadian Geese were trained to their normal migration route by the Canadian ultralight enthusiast Bill Lishman, as shown in the fact-based movie drama ''Fly Away Home''.
 
  
 
== Sexual imprinting ==
 
== Sexual imprinting ==
 +
'''Sexual imprinting''' is the process by which a young animal learns the characteristics of a desirable mate. For example, male [[zebra finch]]es appear to prefer mates with the appearance of the female bird that rears them, rather than mates of their own type (Immelmann 1972).
  
'''Sexual imprinting''' is the process by which a young animal learns the characteristics of a desirable mate. For example, male [[zebra finch]]es appear to prefer mates with the appearance of the female bird that rears them, rather than mates of their own type (Immelmann, 1972).
+
=== Westermarck effect ===
 +
''Reverse'' sexual imprinting is also observed when two people, living in close domestic proximity during the first few years of their lives, become desensitized to sexual attraction and bonding to each other later on. This phenomenon, known as the '''[[Westermarck effect]],''' was discovered by [[anthropology|anthropologist]] [[Edvard Westermarck]]. The Westermarck effect has since been observed in many places and [[culture]]s, including in the [[Israel]]i [[kibbutz]] system, and the ''Shim-pua'' marriage customs of [[Taiwan]], as well as in biologically-related families.  
  
=== Westermarck effect ===
+
When the opposite situation occurs, for example where a brother and sister are raised separately, not knowing about one another, they may find one another highly sexually attractive when they meet as adults—a phenomenon known as genetic sexual attraction. This observation is consistent with the theory that the Westermarck effect evolved to suppress inbreeding.
  
''Reverse'' sexual imprinting is also observed when two people live in close domestic proximity during the first few years of their lives become desensitized to sexual attraction and bonding later on. This phenomenon, known as the '''Westermarck effect''', was discovered by anthropologist [[Edvard Westermarck]]. The Westermarck effect has since been observed in many places and cultures, including in the [[Israel]]i [[kibbutz]] system, and the Shim-pua marriage customs of Taiwan, as well as in biological-related families.  
+
== Evidence of imprinting in neuroscience==
 +
A structure associated with imprinting was located on either side of the [[brain]] in the ''hyperstratium ventrale'' (IMHV). By removing this part of the brain from chicks, Horn (1985) demonstrated that the chicks no longer displayed imprinting tendencies. The ''hyperstratium ventrale'' is a part of the dorsal ventrical ridge (DVR), a common brain structure found in both [[reptile]]s and [[bird]]s. The dorsal ventrical ridge has also been shown to operate in a similar fashion to [[mammal]]s' [[memory]] mechanisms.
  
When this does not occur, for example where a brother and sister are  brought up not knowing about one another, they may find one another highly sexually attractive when they meet as adults— a phenomenon known as genetic sexual attraction. This observation is consistent with the theory that the Westermarck effect evolved to suppress inbreeding.
+
Other research has questioned whether Lorenz's account of imprinting may be somewhat incomplete. Studying the birds in their natural environment, it is argued that Lorenz lacked the objectivity that can be facilitated in a laboratory. The experimental approach of modern research has allowed for understanding of the specific [[learning]] processes that ultimately contribute to the behavior of imprinting.  
  
== Evidence of Imprinting in Neuroscience==
+
With more controlled environments, it has been found that the release of [[endorphin]]s in the [[brain]], providing comforting feedback, is part of the process. Other laboratory evidence suggest that imprinting is a form of associative learning, not entirely [[instinct]]ual, and may take place over longer periods of time and in more species than have been observed in natural settings.
  
In 1985, G. Horn found that a possible structure associated with imprinting was located on either side of the brain in the hyperstratium ventrale (IMHV). By removing the chicks' IMHV, Horn was able to demonstrate that the chicks no longer displayed imprinting tendencies. The hyperstratium ventrale is a part of the dorsal ventrical ridge (DVR) a common brain structure found in both reptiles and birds. The dorsal ventrical ridge also includes a wulst which operates similar to mammals' memory mechanisms.
+
==Application==
 +
Knowledge of the imprinting process has been put to good use in situations where young have been raised in captivity without the presence of adults of their species. Caregivers for birds hatched in captivity have developed techniques to teach them survival skills, based on using the imprinting process to cause the young to identify with humans (often disguised to mimic the colors and patterns of the adult birds) and so follow them, mimicking their behavior.
  
==Recent Research==
+
The [[Italy|Italian]] [[hang gliding|hang glider]] pilot [[Angelo d'Arrigo]] manipulated Lorenz's technique to re-introduce threatened species of [[raptor]]s into the wild. D'Arrigo noted that the flight of a non-motorized hang glider is very similar to the flight patterns of migratory birds, since both use updrafts of thermal currents to gain altitude, permitting soaring flight over long distances. Birds hatched in captivity have no mentor birds to teach them their traditional migratory routes. D'Arrigo hatched chicks under the wing of his glider, and they imprinted on him. Subsequently, he taught the fledglings to fly and to hunt. The young birds followed him not only on the ground (as with Lorenz), but also in the air as he took the path of various migratory routes. He flew across the [[Sahara]] and over the [[Mediterranean Sea]] to [[Sicily]] with [[eagle]]s, from [[Siberia]] to [[Iran]] with a flock of Siberian [[crane]]s, and over [[Mount Everest]] with [[Nepal]]ese eagles. His wife continues his work of re-introducing a breeding pair of [[condor]]s into their South [[America]]n habitat.
  
Recent research contends that the tradition of Lorenz's imprinting may be somewhat incomplete. Studying the birds in their natural environment, it is argued that Lorenz lacked the objectivity that can be facilitated in a laboratory. The experimental approach of modern reseach has allowed for understanding of the specific learning processes that ultimately contribute to the behavior of imprinting.  
+
In a project similar to d'Arrigo's efforts, orphaned Canadian Geese were trained to their normal migration route by the Canadian ultralight enthusiast [[Bill Lishman]], as shown in the fact-based dramatic film, ''Fly Away Home.''
  
With more controlled environments, scientists now claim that imprinting isn't as rapid as previously thought and that it may be a progressive process that relies on the release of endorphins in the brain. It is suggested that imprinting can't be reversed and that there isn't any narrow critical period for imprinting. In 1996, Howard S. Hoffman discovered that imprinting occurs in various species and the specific learning components are more vital than previously thought.
+
==Conclusion==
 +
Imprinting is an intriguing phenomenon, as it is the product of both [[genetics|genetic]] and environmental influences. It seems evident that the young of any creature should possess the ability to ensure their survival. Imprinting is an essential bond between the parent and their newborn, as it enables them to [[learning|learn]] necessary skills and to secure their well-being during potentially dangerous times. Such imprinting is most obvious in nidifugous [[bird]]s and [[reptiles]], and is clearly an advantageous process since young that leave the nest early on are at great risk in unable to find its parents.  
  
==Legacy==
+
Sexual imprinting also can be understood as valuable in the survival of the species, allowing young to identify characteristics of appropriate potential mates.
  
Behavioral imprinting is an intriguing phenomena as it is the product of both genetic and environmental influences. It seems evident that an infant of any animal kingdom should possess the ability to ensure their survival. Imprinting is an essential bond between the parent and their newborn as it enables them to learn necessary skills and to secure their well-being during a potentially dangerous event. Imprinting is most obvious in nidifugous birds and reptiles; however, filial imprinting can also occur in mammals. In the study of child development, the term refers to the process by which a baby learns to distinguish who its mother and father are. The process begins in the womb, when an unborn baby starts to recognize its parent's voices and continues as there is a strong parent-child bond that deepens through lengthier, more complex processes.
+
Imprinting can also occur in [[mammal]]s, albeit in somewhat different forms and involving more prolonged and complex forms of interaction. In the study of [[child development]], the related process by which babies learn to distinguish their mothers, or caregivers, is known as attachment. The process begins in the womb, when an unborn baby starts to recognize the parent's voices, and continues as there is a strong parent-child bond that deepens through lengthier, more complex processes.
  
 
==References==
 
==References==
* Westermarck, E. A. ''The history of human marriage'', 5th edn. London: Macmillan, [[1921]].
+
* Horn, Gabriel. 1985. ''Memory, Imprinting, and the Brain.'' Oxford: Clarendon Press.
* Immelmann, K. (1972) Sexual and other long-term aspects of imprinting in birds and other species. ''Advances in the Study of Behavior'', '''4''', 147–174.
+
* Hoffman, Howard S. 1996. [http://www.animatedsoftware.com/family/howardsh/imprint.htm Imprinting.] Retrieved August 21, 2008.
 +
* Immelmann, K. 1972. Sexual and other long-term aspects of imprinting in birds and other species. ''Advances in the Study of Behavior'' 4: 147–174.
 +
* Westermarck, Edward A. 1921. ''The History of Human Marriage,'' 5th ed. London: Macmillan.
  
 
== External links ==
 
== External links ==
* Cardoso, SH and Sabbatini, RME. [http://www.cerebromente.org.br/n14/experimento/lorenz/index-lorenz.html Learning who is your mother. The behavior of imprinting]. ''Brain & Mind Magazine''.
+
All links retrieved February 27, 2018.
* [http://ecoevo.bio.uci.edu/Faculty/Burley/Burley.html Nancy T. Burley], a researcher into imprinting in zebra finches
+
 
* Debra Lieberman, [[John Tooby]] and [[Leda Cosmides]]. "Does morality have a biological basis? An empirical test of the factors governing moral sentiments relating to incest." Accepted for publication in ''Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B'' November [[2002]]. Available online at [http://citeseer.ist.psu.edu/593584.html Citeseer]
+
* Cardoso, SH and Sabbatini, RME. [http://www.cerebromente.org.br/n14/experimento/lorenz/index-lorenz.html Learning who is your mother. The behavior of imprinting]. ''Brain & Mind Magazine''.  
*[http://www.angelodarrigo.com Angelo d'Arrigo]  personal website
 
  
 +
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]][[Category:Psychology]]
  
  
 
{{Credit1|Imprinting_(psychology)|61463710|}}
 
{{Credit1|Imprinting_(psychology)|61463710|}}

Latest revision as of 00:48, 28 February 2018


Imprinting is used in psychology and ethology to describe any kind of learning that occurs at a particular age or stage of development. A phase-sensitive type of learning, it involves an organism recognizing the characteristics of certain stimuli that are subsequently "imprinted" onto the subject. Most occurrences involve learning to recognize one's parents or potential sexual partners, both of which have value for the survival of the species. Although most commonly observed in birds, this process has been observed in other species, leading to advances not only in the ability to assist various species but also in understanding how similar processes are involved in human development.

Imprinting was initially researched in detail by Konrad Lorenz, through his naturalistic studies of geese. The image of Lorenz followed by a family of goslings became familiar to many. Applications of this knowledge have been put to good use, ensuring that young raised in captivity learn survival skills, and, thus, can function in a relatively natural type of situation or even be re-introduced into the wild.

Outline of the theory

The most commonly found form of imprinting is known as "filial imprinting." Filial imprinting occurs when a young animal recognizes the characteristics of its parent. It is most obvious in nidifugous birds (who leave the nest shortly after hatching) and reptiles, who imprint on their parents.

Filial imprinting was first reported in domestic chickens, by the nineteenth century amateur biologist Douglas Spalding. It was rediscovered by the early ethologist Oskar Heinroth, and studied scientifically and popularized by his disciple, Konrad Lorenz in his work with graylag geese. Lorenz demonstrated how incubator-hatched geese would imprint on the first suitable, moving stimulus they saw within what he called a "critical period" of about 36 hours shortly after hatching. Being present with the goslings during their hatching, Lorenz found that the goslings would imprint on himself. As a result of this, he is often depicted being followed by a gaggle of geese who had imprinted on him.

Sexual imprinting

Sexual imprinting is the process by which a young animal learns the characteristics of a desirable mate. For example, male zebra finches appear to prefer mates with the appearance of the female bird that rears them, rather than mates of their own type (Immelmann 1972).

Westermarck effect

Reverse sexual imprinting is also observed when two people, living in close domestic proximity during the first few years of their lives, become desensitized to sexual attraction and bonding to each other later on. This phenomenon, known as the Westermarck effect, was discovered by anthropologist Edvard Westermarck. The Westermarck effect has since been observed in many places and cultures, including in the Israeli kibbutz system, and the Shim-pua marriage customs of Taiwan, as well as in biologically-related families.

When the opposite situation occurs, for example where a brother and sister are raised separately, not knowing about one another, they may find one another highly sexually attractive when they meet as adults—a phenomenon known as genetic sexual attraction. This observation is consistent with the theory that the Westermarck effect evolved to suppress inbreeding.

Evidence of imprinting in neuroscience

A structure associated with imprinting was located on either side of the brain in the hyperstratium ventrale (IMHV). By removing this part of the brain from chicks, Horn (1985) demonstrated that the chicks no longer displayed imprinting tendencies. The hyperstratium ventrale is a part of the dorsal ventrical ridge (DVR), a common brain structure found in both reptiles and birds. The dorsal ventrical ridge has also been shown to operate in a similar fashion to mammals' memory mechanisms.

Other research has questioned whether Lorenz's account of imprinting may be somewhat incomplete. Studying the birds in their natural environment, it is argued that Lorenz lacked the objectivity that can be facilitated in a laboratory. The experimental approach of modern research has allowed for understanding of the specific learning processes that ultimately contribute to the behavior of imprinting.

With more controlled environments, it has been found that the release of endorphins in the brain, providing comforting feedback, is part of the process. Other laboratory evidence suggest that imprinting is a form of associative learning, not entirely instinctual, and may take place over longer periods of time and in more species than have been observed in natural settings.

Application

Knowledge of the imprinting process has been put to good use in situations where young have been raised in captivity without the presence of adults of their species. Caregivers for birds hatched in captivity have developed techniques to teach them survival skills, based on using the imprinting process to cause the young to identify with humans (often disguised to mimic the colors and patterns of the adult birds) and so follow them, mimicking their behavior.

The Italian hang glider pilot Angelo d'Arrigo manipulated Lorenz's technique to re-introduce threatened species of raptors into the wild. D'Arrigo noted that the flight of a non-motorized hang glider is very similar to the flight patterns of migratory birds, since both use updrafts of thermal currents to gain altitude, permitting soaring flight over long distances. Birds hatched in captivity have no mentor birds to teach them their traditional migratory routes. D'Arrigo hatched chicks under the wing of his glider, and they imprinted on him. Subsequently, he taught the fledglings to fly and to hunt. The young birds followed him not only on the ground (as with Lorenz), but also in the air as he took the path of various migratory routes. He flew across the Sahara and over the Mediterranean Sea to Sicily with eagles, from Siberia to Iran with a flock of Siberian cranes, and over Mount Everest with Nepalese eagles. His wife continues his work of re-introducing a breeding pair of condors into their South American habitat.

In a project similar to d'Arrigo's efforts, orphaned Canadian Geese were trained to their normal migration route by the Canadian ultralight enthusiast Bill Lishman, as shown in the fact-based dramatic film, Fly Away Home.

Conclusion

Imprinting is an intriguing phenomenon, as it is the product of both genetic and environmental influences. It seems evident that the young of any creature should possess the ability to ensure their survival. Imprinting is an essential bond between the parent and their newborn, as it enables them to learn necessary skills and to secure their well-being during potentially dangerous times. Such imprinting is most obvious in nidifugous birds and reptiles, and is clearly an advantageous process since young that leave the nest early on are at great risk in unable to find its parents.

Sexual imprinting also can be understood as valuable in the survival of the species, allowing young to identify characteristics of appropriate potential mates.

Imprinting can also occur in mammals, albeit in somewhat different forms and involving more prolonged and complex forms of interaction. In the study of child development, the related process by which babies learn to distinguish their mothers, or caregivers, is known as attachment. The process begins in the womb, when an unborn baby starts to recognize the parent's voices, and continues as there is a strong parent-child bond that deepens through lengthier, more complex processes.

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Horn, Gabriel. 1985. Memory, Imprinting, and the Brain. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  • Hoffman, Howard S. 1996. Imprinting. Retrieved August 21, 2008.
  • Immelmann, K. 1972. Sexual and other long-term aspects of imprinting in birds and other species. Advances in the Study of Behavior 4: 147–174.
  • Westermarck, Edward A. 1921. The History of Human Marriage, 5th ed. London: Macmillan.

External links

All links retrieved February 27, 2018.


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.