Difference between revisions of "Grid illusion" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
(Copied from wikipedia)
(started)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Claimed}}
+
{{Claimed}}{{Started}}
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
  
 +
[[Image:Hermann grid illusion.svg|thumb|256px|right|A Hermann grid illusion. Shape position and color [[contrast (vision)|contrast]] converge to produce the illusion of grey blobs at the intersections.]]A '''grid illusion''' is any kind of [[grid]] that deceives a person's vision. The two most common types of grid illusions are '''Hermann grid illusions''' and '''Scintillating grid illusions'''.
 +
==Hermann grid illusion==
 +
The '''Hermann grid illusion''' is an [[optical illusion]] reported by [[Ludimar Hermann]] in 1870 while, incidentally, reading [[John Tyndall]]'s ''Sound''. The illusion is characterised by "ghostlike" grey blobs perceived at the intersections of a white (or light-colored) grid on a black background. The grey blobs disappear when looking directly at an intersection.
  
[[Image:Grid illusion.svg|thumb|256px|right|An example of the Scintillating grid illusion]]
 
A '''grid illusion''' is any kind of [[grid]] that deceives a person's vision. The two most common types of grid illusions are '''Scintillating grid illusions''' and '''Hermann grid illusions'''.
 
 
==Scintillating grid illusion==
 
==Scintillating grid illusion==
The '''Scintillating grid illusion''' is an [[optical illusion]] when dots seem to appear and disappear at the intersections of two lines crossing each other vertically and horizontally. When a person keeps his or her eyes directly on a single intersection, the dot does not appear. A variation of the Scintillating illusion is the Hermann grid illusion (see section below). In the picture on the right, a person should see white dots turn black and then turning white again very fast.  The dots disappear if one is too close or too far from the image.
+
[[Image:Grid illusion.svg|thumb|256px|left|An example of the scintillating grid illusion. Dark dots seem to appear and disappear at intersections]]
 
+
The '''scintillating grid illusion''' is an [[optical illusion]] discovered by E. Lingelbach in 1994, that is usually considered a variation of the Hermann grid illusion (see section below).  
==Hermann grid illusion==
 
[[Image:Hermann grid illusion.svg|thumb|202px|left|A Hermann grid illusion. Shape position and color [[contrast (vision)|contrast]] converge to produce the illusion of spots at the intersections.]]
 
The '''Hermann grid illusion''' is an [[optical illusion]] reported by [[Ludimar Hermann]] in [[1870]] while, incidentally, reading [[John Tyndall]]'s ''Sound''. It is very similar to the Scintillating grid illusion.  
 
  
Like the Scintillating grid illusion, when looking at a grid of black squares on a white (or light-colored) background, one will have the impression that there are "ghostlike" grey blobs at the intersections of the white lines. The grey blobs disappear when looking directly at an intersection.
+
It is constructed by superimposing white discs on the intersections of orthogonal gray bars on a black background. Dark dots seem to appear and disappear rapidly at random intersections, hence the label “scintillating.” When a person keeps his or her eyes directly on a single intersection, the dot does not appear. The dots disappear if one is too close or too far from the image.
  
[[Image:White grid illusion.svg|thumb|218px|right|Another type of Hermann grid illusion.]]
+
==Differences between the scintillating and Hermann grid illusions==
 +
The difference between the Hermann grid illusion and the scintillating illusion is that scintillating illusions have dots already in place at the intersection, whereas there are no dots already in place at the intersections of Hermann grid illusions. Since they are so similar, the two names are commonly used interchangeably. But the scintillating illusion does not occur with an isolated intersection, as in the case of the Hermann grid; observations suggest that a minimum of 3 × 3 evenly spaced intersections with superimposed discs are required to produce the effect. This requirement suggests the participation of global processes of the kind proposed for the linking and grouping of features in an image, in addition to local processes.  
  
==Differences between the Scintillating and Hermann grid illusions==
+
[[Image:White grid illusion.svg|thumb|200px|right|A white-background version of the scintillating grid illusion.]]
The difference between the Hermann grid illusion and the Scintillating illusion is that Scintillating illusions have dots already in place at the intersection, whereas there are no dots already in place at the intersections of Hermann grid illusions. However, since they are so similar, the two names are commonly switched around.
+
==The cause of both scintillating and Hermann grid illusions==
 +
The effect of both optical illusions is commonly (and falsely) explained by a neural process called [[lateral inhibition]].  The intensity at a point in the visual system is not simply the result of a single [[sensory receptor|receptor]], but the result of a group of receptors called a [[receptive field]].
  
==The cause of both Scintillating and Hermann grid illusions==
+
A [[retinal ganglion cell]] pools the inputs of several [[photoreceptors]] over an area of [[retina]], the area covered by the photoreceptors is the ganglion cells "receptive field." In the center of the receptive field the individual photoreceptors ''excite'' the ganglion cell when they detect increased luminance. The photoreceptors in the surrounding area ''inhibit'' the ganglion cellThus, since a point at an intersection is surrounded by more intensity than a point at the middle of a line, the intersection appears darker due to the increased inhibition.
The effect of the optical illusion is commonly (and falsely) explained by a neural process called [[lateral inhibition]].  The intensity at a point in the visual system is not simply the result of a single [[sensory receptor|receptor]], but the result of a group of receptors called a [[receptive field]].
 
  
In the center of the receptive field, the receptors act ''excitatory'' on the resulting signal, and the receptors in the surrounding area act ''inhibitory'' on the signal. Thus, since a point at an intersection is surrounded by more intensity than a point at the middle of a line, the intersection appears darker. In a person's eyes, the nerve cells of the retina associate and interact with each other, which results in the illusion that there are dots, when there really aren't.
+
This explanation has recently been successfully challenged by Janos Geier (see the [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/lum_herGridCurved/index.html interactive counter example]) using a "wavy" version of the Hermann grid in which no dots appear.
  
This explanation has recently been successfully challenged by Janos Geier (see the [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/lum_herGridCurved/index.html interactive counter example]).
+
==Notes==
  
 +
<references />
 
==References==
 
==References==
<!-- ----------------------------------------------------------
 
  See http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wikipedia:Footnotes for a
 
  discussion of different citation methods and how to generate
 
  footnotes using the <ref>, </ref> and  <reference /> tags
 
----------------------------------------------------------- —>
 
<div class="references-small">
 
<references />
 
</div>
 
  
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
Line 45: Line 37:
  
  
 
+
{{Credits|Grid_illusion|149108681|}}
 
 
{{Credits|Grid_illusion|115267816|}}
 

Revision as of 00:14, 6 September 2007


File:Hermann grid illusion.svg
A Hermann grid illusion. Shape position and color contrast converge to produce the illusion of grey blobs at the intersections.

A grid illusion is any kind of grid that deceives a person's vision. The two most common types of grid illusions are Hermann grid illusions and Scintillating grid illusions.

Hermann grid illusion

The Hermann grid illusion is an optical illusion reported by Ludimar Hermann in 1870 while, incidentally, reading John Tyndall's Sound. The illusion is characterised by "ghostlike" grey blobs perceived at the intersections of a white (or light-colored) grid on a black background. The grey blobs disappear when looking directly at an intersection.

Scintillating grid illusion

An example of the scintillating grid illusion. Dark dots seem to appear and disappear at intersections

The scintillating grid illusion is an optical illusion discovered by E. Lingelbach in 1994, that is usually considered a variation of the Hermann grid illusion (see section below).

It is constructed by superimposing white discs on the intersections of orthogonal gray bars on a black background. Dark dots seem to appear and disappear rapidly at random intersections, hence the label “scintillating.” When a person keeps his or her eyes directly on a single intersection, the dot does not appear. The dots disappear if one is too close or too far from the image.

Differences between the scintillating and Hermann grid illusions

The difference between the Hermann grid illusion and the scintillating illusion is that scintillating illusions have dots already in place at the intersection, whereas there are no dots already in place at the intersections of Hermann grid illusions. Since they are so similar, the two names are commonly used interchangeably. But the scintillating illusion does not occur with an isolated intersection, as in the case of the Hermann grid; observations suggest that a minimum of 3 × 3 evenly spaced intersections with superimposed discs are required to produce the effect. This requirement suggests the participation of global processes of the kind proposed for the linking and grouping of features in an image, in addition to local processes.

A white-background version of the scintillating grid illusion.

The cause of both scintillating and Hermann grid illusions

The effect of both optical illusions is commonly (and falsely) explained by a neural process called lateral inhibition. The intensity at a point in the visual system is not simply the result of a single receptor, but the result of a group of receptors called a receptive field.

A retinal ganglion cell pools the inputs of several photoreceptors over an area of retina, the area covered by the photoreceptors is the ganglion cells "receptive field." In the center of the receptive field the individual photoreceptors excite the ganglion cell when they detect increased luminance. The photoreceptors in the surrounding area inhibit the ganglion cell. Thus, since a point at an intersection is surrounded by more intensity than a point at the middle of a line, the intersection appears darker due to the increased inhibition.

This explanation has recently been successfully challenged by Janos Geier (see the interactive counter example) using a "wavy" version of the Hermann grid in which no dots appear.

Notes

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

External links


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.