Difference between revisions of "Hawthorne effect" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
 
(25 intermediate revisions by 7 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Claimed}}{{Started}}{{Contracted}}
+
{{Submitted}}{{Images OK}}{{Approved}}{{Paid}}{{Copyedited}}
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
[[Category:Sociology]]
 
[[Category:Sociology]]
  
 +
The '''Hawthorne effect''' refers to a phenomenon which is thought to occur when people observed during a research study temporarily change their behavior or performance (this can also be referred to as [[demand characteristics]]). Others have broadened the definition to mean that people’s behavior and performance change following any new or increased attention. The Hawthorne studies have had a dramatic effect on [[management]] in organizations and understanding the impact of different factors in the workplace.
  
The '''Hawthorne effect''' refers to a phenomenon which is thought to occur when people observed during a research study temporarily change their behavior or performance (this can also be referred to as [[demand characteristics]]). Others have broadened the definition to mean that people’s behavior and performance change following any new or increased attention. The Hawthorne studies have had a dramatic effect on management in organizations and how people react to different situations.
+
The purpose of the original experiments was to study the effect of [[light]]ing on workers’ productivity. When researchers found that productivity almost always increased after a change in illumination, no matter what the level of illumination was, a second set of experiments began, supervised by [[Harvard University]] professors [[Elton Mayo]], [[Fritz Roethlisberger]], and William J. Dickson.  
  
The term gets its name from a factory called the [[Hawthorne Works]],<ref>[http://www.assemblymag.com/CDA/Archives/0cdaaa2e0d5c9010VgnVCM100000f932a8c0____ "The Hawthorne Works" from Assembly Magazine]</ref> where a series of experiments on factory workers were carried out between 1924 and 1932.
+
They experimented on other types of changes in the working environment, using a study group of five young women. Again, no matter the change in conditions, the women nearly always produced more. The researchers reported that they had accidentally found a way to increase productivity. The effect was an important milestone in [[applied psychology|industrial and organizational psychology]] and in [[organizational behavior]], bringing awareness to the impact of social factors and [[socialization]] in the workplace. In this way, management and researchers alike recognize that people are essentially social beings, and their relationships with others are highly significant in everything that they do.
 +
{{toc}}
 +
However, some researchers have questioned the validity of the effect because of the experiments’ design and faulty interpretations. Thus, the Hawthorne effect refers not only to the findings relating to productivity, but also to the issue of people's behavior being changed by the experimental study itself, which confounds the factors under investigation.
  
There were many types of experiments conducted on the employees, but the purpose of the original ones was to study the effect of lighting on workers’ productivity. When researchers found that productivity almost always increased after a change in illumination, no matter what the level of illumination was, a second set of experiments began, supervised by Harvard University professors [[Elton Mayo]], [[Fritz Roethlisberger]] and William J. Dickson.
+
== Definition ==
  
They experimented on other types of changes in the working environment, using a study group of five young women.  Again, no matter the change in conditions, the women nearly always produced more. The researchers reported that they had accidentally found a way to increase productivity. The effect was an important milestone in [[industrial and organizational psychology]] and in [[organizational behavior]]. However, some researchers have questioned the validity of the effect because of the experiments’ design and faulty interpretations.
+
The '''Hawthorne experiments''' sought to determine the [[motivation|motivating]] factors behind success and productivity in the work place. The hypothesis focused on [[economics|economic]] factors being the biggest contributor to work place success.<ref>Stephen Draper. 2006.[http://www.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/hawth.html ''Finding The Hawthorne, Pygmalion, Placebo and Other Effects of Expectation.''] University of Glasgow. Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref> However, the results suggested different factors were important.  
  
== Definitions ==
+
Social factors of the employment itself were found to be the biggest motivator in job performance, mainly how much attention was being paid to individual employees. The Hawthorne effect is, essentially, the positive effect on a person or group's behavior from knowingly being watched. It is a positive effect because when a person suspects they are being observed, they are more prone to act at a higher level of efficiency than they normally act.<ref> Lester Breslow and John Last. 2002. ''Encyclopedia of Public Health.'' Macmillan Reference. ISBN 0028653548. Retrieved August 14, 2007. </ref> Another way to interpret this is the failure on the part of the experimenters to identify that their observation or experiment is motivating the subjects to act differently than they normally do; they act in a way that they think the scientists want them to act.<ref>H. M. Parsons, 1974. [http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/abstract/183/4128/922 ''What Happened at Hawthorne?''] ''Science Journal'' 183 (4128)(March 8, 1974): 922-932. Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref>
There are many different ways to define and examine the Hawthorne effect. One definition is an experimental effect in the direction expected but not for the reason expected. For example, a significant positive effect that turns out to have no causal basis in the theoretical motivation for the intervention, but is apparently due to the effect on the participants of knowing themselves to be studied in connection with the outcomes measured. <ref>[http://www.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/hawth.html ''Finding The Hawthorne, Pygmalion, Placebo and Other Effects of Expectation.''] Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref>  
 
 
 
Another definition can be described as the confounding that occurs if experimenters fail to realize how the consequences of subjects' performance affect what subjects do.<ref name=Parsons> Parsons, H.M. 1974. [http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/abstract/183/4128/922 ''What Happened at Hawthorne?''] Science Journal. Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref>
 
 
 
It has also been suggested that the Hawthorne effect shows that peoples performance or behavior may improve or change during a study because they are aware they are being watched. <ref> [http://www.cs.unc.edu/~stotts/204/nohawth.html The Hawthorne Defect: Persistence of a Flawed Theory] Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref>
 
 
 
A final evaluation of the Hawthorne effect demonstrates that people will respond positively to any novel change in a work environment. <ref> Jex, S.M. 2002. ''Organizational Psychology: A Scientist-Practitioner Approach.'' John Wiley and Sons. New York. Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref>
 
  
 
== History of the Hawthorne Experiments==
 
== History of the Hawthorne Experiments==
Like the Hawthorne effect, the definition of the Hawthorne experiments also varies. Most industrial/occupational psychology and organizational behavior textbooks refer to the illumination studies, and usually to the relay assembly test room experiments and the bank wiring room experiments. Only occasionally are the rest of the studies mentioned.<ref>[http://www.siop.org/tip/backissues/Jan%2004/pdf/413_023to039.pdf What We Teach Students About the Hawthorne Studies: A Review of Content Within a Sample of Introductory I-O and OB Textbooks]</ref>.
+
Like the Hawthorne effect, the definition of the Hawthorne experiments also varies. The Hawthorne Works, located in Cicero, [[Illinois]], just outside of [[Chicago]], belonged to the [[Western Electric Company]], and the studies were funded by the National Research Council of the National Academy of Sciences at the behest of [[General Electric]], the largest manufacturer of [[light bulb]]s in the United States.<ref>Edwin Lee Makamson. 2000.  [http://www.mgmtguru.com/mgt301/301_Lecture1Page10.htm ''The Hawthorne Experiments: Management Takes A New Direction.] ''Management Guru.'' Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref> The purpose was to find the optimum level of lighting for productivity.
  
The Hawthorne Works, located in Cicero, Illinois, and just outside of Chicago, belonged to the [[Western Electric Company]], and the studies were funded by the National Research Council of the National Academy of Sciences at the behest of [[General Electric]], the largest manufacturer of light bulbs in the United States. <ref> [http://www.mgmtguru.com/mgt301/301_Lecture1Page10.htm ''The Hawthorne Experiments: Management Takes A New Direction.] Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref> The purpose was to find the optimum level of lighting for productivity.
+
Most [[applied psychology|industrial and occupational psychology]] and organizational behavior textbooks refer to the illumination studies, and usually to the relay assembly test room experiments and the bank wiring room experiments. Only occasionally are the rest of the studies mentioned.<ref>R. Olson, J. Verley, L. Santos, and C. Salas. What We Teach Students About the Hawthorne Studies: A Review of Content Within a Sample of Introductory I-O and OB Textbooks Santa Clara Univeristy.</ref>
  
 
=== Illumination Experiments ===
 
=== Illumination Experiments ===
  
During two and a half years from 1924 to 1927, a series of '''illumination experiments''' using different study groups were conducted by the industrial engineers of Western Electric Company Works in Cicero; Illinois.<ref> Dickson, W.J. Roethlisberger, F.J. 1939. ''Management and the Worker.'' Harvard University Press. Cambridge, MA. ISBN: 0415279879. Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref>
+
During two and a half years from 1924 to 1927, a series of illumination experiments using different study groups were conducted by the industrial engineers of Western Electric Company Works in Cicero; Illinois.<ref>W.J. Dickson and F.J. Roethlisberger, 1939. ''Management and the Worker.'' (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0415279871). Retrieved July 31, 2007. </ref>
 
    
 
    
 
* Study 1a: In the first experiment, there was no control group. The researchers experimented on three different departments; all showed an increase of productivity, whether illumination increased or decreased.
 
* Study 1a: In the first experiment, there was no control group. The researchers experimented on three different departments; all showed an increase of productivity, whether illumination increased or decreased.
* Study 1b: A control group had no change in lighting, while the experimental group got a sequence of increasing light levels. Both groups substantially increased production, and there was no difference between the groups. This naturally piqued the researchers' curiosity.
+
* Study 1b: A control group had no change in lighting, while the experimental group got a sequence of increasing light levels. Both groups substantially increased production, and there was no difference between the groups. This naturally piqued the researchers' curiosity.
* Study 1c: The researchers decided to see what would happen if they decreased lighting. The control group got stable illumination; the other got a sequence of decreasing levels. Surprisingly, both groups steadily increased production until finally the light in experimental group got so low that they protested and production fell off.
+
* Study 1c: The researchers decided to see what would happen if they decreased lighting. The control group had stable illumination; the other experienced a sequence of decreasing levels. Surprisingly, both groups steadily increased production until finally the light in experimental group became so low that they protested and production fell off.
* Study 1d: This was conducted on two women only. Their production stayed constant under widely varying light levels. It was found that if the experimenter said bright was good, they said they preferred the light; the brighter they believed it to be, the more they liked it. The same was true when he said dimmer was good. If they were deceived about a change, they said they preferred it. Researchers concluded that their preference on lighting level was completely subjective - if they were told it was good, they believed it was good and preferred it, and vice versa.
+
* Study 1d: This was conducted on two women only. Their production stayed constant under widely varying light levels. It was found that if the experimenter said bright was good, they said they preferred the light; the brighter they believed it to be, the more they liked it. The same was true when he said dimmer was good. If they were deceived about a change, they said they preferred it. Researchers concluded that their preference on lighting level was completely subjective - if they were told it was good, they believed it was good and preferred it, and vice versa.
  
At this point, researchers of Western Electric realized that something else besides lighting was affecting productivity. They suspected that the supervision of the researchers had some effect, so they ended the illumination experiments in 1927.
+
At this point, researchers of Western Electric realized that something else besides lighting was affecting productivity. They suspected that the supervision of the researchers had some effect, so they ended the illumination experiments in 1927.
  
During 1927, a Harvard professor, Elton Mayo, along with his associates, joined the study as consultants. The experiments started again and lasted through to 1932. These experiments consisted of redesigning jobs, changing workday and workweek lengths, additional rest times and studying the factor of wage changes.
+
During 1927, a [[Harvard]] professor, [[Elton Mayo]], along with his associates, joined the study as consultants. The experiments started again and lasted through to 1932. These experiments consisted of redesigning jobs, changing workday and workweek lengths, additional rest times, and studying the factor of [[wage]] changes.
  
 
=== Relay-Assembly Experiments ===
 
=== Relay-Assembly Experiments ===
  
In the '''relay-assembly experiments''', the researchers wanted to identify how other variables could affect productivity. They chose two women as test subjects and asked them to choose four other workers to join the test group. Together the women worked in a separate room over the course of five years (1927-1932) assembling telephone relays.  
+
In the relay-assembly experiments, the researchers wanted to identify how other variables could affect productivity. They chose two women as test subjects and asked them to choose four other workers to join the test group. Together the women worked in a separate room over the course of five years (1927-1932) assembling telephone relays.<ref>Austin Weber. 2002. [http://www.assemblymag.com/CDA/Archives/0cdaaa2e0d5c9010VgnVCM100000f932a8c0____ ''The Hawthorne Works.''] ''Assembly Magazine''. Retrieved August 5, 2007. </ref>
  
 
Output was measured mechanically by counting how many finished relays each dropped down a chute. This measuring began in secret two weeks before moving the women to an experiment room and continued throughout the study. In the experiment room, they had a supervisor who discussed changes with them and at times used their suggestions. Then the researchers spent five years measuring how different variables impacted the group's and individuals' productivity. Some of the variables were:
 
Output was measured mechanically by counting how many finished relays each dropped down a chute. This measuring began in secret two weeks before moving the women to an experiment room and continued throughout the study. In the experiment room, they had a supervisor who discussed changes with them and at times used their suggestions. Then the researchers spent five years measuring how different variables impacted the group's and individuals' productivity. Some of the variables were:
 
* changing the pay rules so that the group was paid for overall group production, not individual production
 
* changing the pay rules so that the group was paid for overall group production, not individual production
* giving two 5-minute breaks (after a discussion with them on the best length of time), and then changing to two 10-minute breaks (not their preference). Productivity increased, but when they received six 5-minute rests, they disliked it and reduced output.
+
* giving two five-minute breaks (after a discussion with them on the best length of time), and then changing to two ten-minute breaks (not their preference). Productivity increased, but when they received six five-minute rests, they disliked it and reduced output.
 
* providing food during the breaks
 
* providing food during the breaks
 
* shortening the day by 30 minutes (output went up); shortening it more (output per hour went up, but overall output decreased); returning to the earlier condition (where output peaked).  
 
* shortening the day by 30 minutes (output went up); shortening it more (output per hour went up, but overall output decreased); returning to the earlier condition (where output peaked).  
  
Changing a variable usually increased productivity, even if the variable was just a change back to the original condition. However it is said that this is the natural processes of the human being to adapt to the environment without knowing the objective of the experiment occurring. Researchers concluded that the workers worked harder because they thought that they were being monitored individually.
+
Changing a variable usually increased productivity, even if the variable was just a change back to the original condition. Researchers concluded that the workers worked harder because they thought that they were being monitored individually. However this can be interpreted as the natural processes of the human being to adapt to the environment without reference to the experiment.  
  
 
Researchers hypothesized that choosing one's own coworkers, working as a group, being treated as special (as evidenced by working in a separate room), and having a sympathetic supervisor were the real reasons for the productivity increase. One interpretation, mainly due to Mayo, was that "the six individuals became a team and the team gave itself wholeheartedly and spontaneously to cooperation in the experiment." (There was a second relay assembly test room study whose results were not as significant as the first experiment.)
 
Researchers hypothesized that choosing one's own coworkers, working as a group, being treated as special (as evidenced by working in a separate room), and having a sympathetic supervisor were the real reasons for the productivity increase. One interpretation, mainly due to Mayo, was that "the six individuals became a team and the team gave itself wholeheartedly and spontaneously to cooperation in the experiment." (There was a second relay assembly test room study whose results were not as significant as the first experiment.)
Line 56: Line 52:
 
=== Bank-Wiring Room Experiments ===
 
=== Bank-Wiring Room Experiments ===
  
The purpose of the '''bank wiring room experiments''' was to find out how payment incentives would affect group productivity. The surprising result was that they had no effect. Ironically, this contradicted the Hawthorne effect: although the workers were receiving special attention, it didn’t affect their behavior or productivity. However, the informal group dynamics studied were a new milestone in organizational behavior.
+
The purpose of the bank wiring room experiments was to find out how payment incentives would affect group productivity. The surprising result was that they had no effect. Ironically, this contradicted the Hawthorne effect: although the workers were receiving special attention, it did not affect their behavior or productivity. However, the informal group dynamics studied were a new milestone in organizational behavior.<ref>Ibid.</ref>
  
The study was conducted by Mayo and [[W. Lloyd Warner]] between 1931 and 1932 on a group of 14 men who put together telephone switching equipment. The researchers found that although the workers were paid according to individual productivity, productivity did not go up because the men were afraid that the company would lower the base rate. The men also formed cliques, ostracized coworkers, and created a social hierarchy that was only partly related to the difference in their jobs. The cliques served to control group members and to manage bosses; when bosses asked questions, clique members gave the same responses, even if they were untrue.
+
The study was conducted by Mayo and [[W. Lloyd Warner]] between 1931 and 1932 on a group of 14 men who put together telephone switching equipment. The researchers found that although the workers were paid according to individual productivity, productivity did not go up because the men were afraid that the company would lower the base rate. The men also formed cliques, ostracized coworkers, and created a social hierarchy that was only partly related to the difference in their jobs. The cliques served to control group members and to manage bosses; when bosses asked questions, clique members gave the same responses, even if they were untrue.
  
== Literature and Research ==
+
== Evaluation ==
 
 
The experiments were performed well enough to establish that there were large effects due to causal factors other than the simple physical ones the experiments had originally been designed to study. The output ("dependent") variables were human work, and we can expect that educational effects to be similar (but it is not so obvious that medical effects would be). The experiments stand as a warning about simple experiments on human participants as if they were only material systems. There is less certainty about the nature of the surprise factor, other than it certainly depended on the mental states of the participants: their knowledge, beliefs, and other predispositions. Candidate causes are:
 
 
 
#Material factors, as originally studied e.g. illumination, ...
 
#Motivation or goals e.g. piecework, ...
 
#Feedback: one can not learn skill without good feedback. Simply providing proper feedback can be a big factor. This can often be a side effect of an experiment, and good ethical practice promotes this further. Yet perhaps providing the feedback with nothing else may be a powerful factor.
 
#The attention of experimenters.
 
 
 
Parsons <ref name=Parsons/> implies that "attention of experimenters" might be a factor as a major heading in our thinking, but as a cause can be reduced to a mixture of "motivation or goals" and "feedback." That is: people might take on pleasing the experimenter as a goal, at least if it doesn't conflict with any other motive; but also, improving their performance by improving their skill will be dependent on getting feedback on their performance, and an experiment may give them this for the first time. So you often won't see any Hawthorne effect — only when it turns out that with the attention came either usable feedback or a change in motivation.
 
 
 
Adair <ref name=Adair> Adair, John. Huynh, Cam-Loi. Sharpe, Donald. 1989. [http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0034-6543(198922)59%3A2%3C215%3AHCPIEE%3E2.0.CO%3B2-E Hawthorne Control Procedures in Educational Experiments: A Reconsideration of Their Use and Effectiveness] Review of Educational Research. Retrieved August 4, 2007. </ref> warns of gross factual inaccuracy in most secondary publications on Hawthorne effect. And that many studies failed to find it, but some did. He argues that we should look at it as a variant of Orne's (1973) experimental demand characteristics. So for Adair, the issue is that an experimental effect depends on the participants' interpretation of the situation; that this may not be at all like the experimenter's interpretation and the right method is to do post-experimental interviews in depth and with care to discover participants' interpretations. So he thinks it is not awareness per se; nor special attention per se; but you have to investigate participants' interpretation in order to discover if/how the experimental conditions interact with the participants' goals (in participants' view). This can affect whether participants' believe something, if they act on it or don't see it as in their interest, etc.
 
 
 
Its interpretation in management research
 
The research was and is relevant firstly in the 'Human Resources Management' movement. The discovery of the effect was most immediately a blow to those hoping to apply the behavioral sciences to manipulate workers in the interest of management.
 
 
 
Other interpretations it has been linked to are: Durkheim's 'anomie' concept; the Weberian model of a system of bureaucratic formal organization; a system of informal group relations, as in the interpretation of Mayo and his followers; a system of power, of class antagonisms.
 
  
Social scientist Harry Braverman suggests that the Hawthorne tests were based on behaviorist psychology and were supposed to confirm that workers performance could be predicted by pre-hire testing. However, the Hawthorne study showed "that the performance of workers had little relation to ability and in fact often bore a reverse relation to test scores...." What the studies really showed was that the workplace was not "a system of bureaucratic formal organization on the [[Max Weber|Weberian model]], nor a system of informal group relations, as in the interpretation of Mayo and his followers but rather a system of power, of class antagonisms." This discovery was a blow to those hoping to apply the behavioral sciences to manipulate workers in the interest of management. <ref> Braverman, Harry. 1998. [http://www.monthlyreview.org/labormon.htm ''Labor and Monopoly Capital.''] ISBN 0-85345-940-1. Retrieved August 4, 2007. </ref>
+
The Hawthorne effect is notable from several perspectives of social awareness and [[cognition]]. It seems clear that in some cases there is a large effect that experimenters did not anticipate, that is due to participants' reactions to the experiment itself. So as a methodological heuristic, it is useful, but as an exact predictor of effects, it is not: it is difficult to ascertain the Hawthorne effect in every situation. To understand when and why we will see a Hawthorne or experimenter effect, we need more detailed considerations.
  
What may be wrong about the quoted dismissiveness is that there was not one study, but three illumination experiments, and 4 other experiments: only one of these seven is alluded to. What is right is that a) there certainly are significant criticisms of the method that can be made and b) most subsequent writing shows a predisposition to believe in the Hawthorne effect, and a failure to read the actual original studies. <ref> Campbell, Donald. Stanley, Julian. 1963. ''Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research.'' Houghton Mifflin Company. ISBN 978-0395307878. Retrieved August 4, 2007. </ref>
+
From another perspective, it is suggested that the most important (though not the only) aspect of this is how the participants interpret the situation.<ref>John Adair, Donald Sharpe, and Cam-Loi Huynh. 1989. Hawthorne Control Procedures in Educational Experiments: A Reconsideration of Their Use and Effectiveness ''Review of Educational Research'' 59 (2)(Summer, 1989): 215-228.</ref> This is important because factory workers, students, and most experimental participants are doing things at the request of the experimenter. What they do depends on what their personal goals are, how they understand the task requested, whether they want to please the experimenter and/or whether they see this task as impinging on other interests and goals they hold, what they think the experimenter really wants. Besides all those issues that determine their goals and intentions in the experiment, further aspects of how they understand the situation can be important by affecting what they believe about the effects of their actions.
  
== Criticisms ==
+
Another issue is the question of what direct causal factors might be involved. These could include material ones that are intended by the experimenter, feedback that an experiment might make available to the participants, or changes to goals, motivation, and beliefs about action effects induced by the experimental situation.<ref>Donald Campbell, and Julian Stanley. 1963. ''Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research.'' (Houghton Mifflin Company. ISBN 978-0395307878). Retrieved August 4, 2007. </ref>
Parsons argues that in the studies, there is at least some evidence that the following factors were potent: <ref name=Parsons/>
 
  
#Rest periods
+
=== Criticism ===
#[[Learning]], given feedback i.e. skill acquisition
 
#Piecework pay where an individual does get more pay for more work, without counter-pressures (e.g. believing that management will just lower pay rates).
 
  
Clearly the variables the experimenters manipulated were neither the only nor the dominant causes of productivity changes. One interpretation, mainly due to Mayo, was that "the six individuals became a team and the team gave itself wholeheartedly and spontaneously to cooperation in the experiment." In 1955 Landsberger reinterpreted the experimental outcomes as the more general result of being observed and labeled this as the Hawthorne effect. <ref> Landsberger, Henry A. 1958. [http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-7732(195905)37%3A4%3C361%3AHR%3E2.0.CO%3B2-A ''Hawthorne Revisited.''] Ithaca, NY. Cornell University. Social Forces. Retrieved August 5, 2007. </ref>  
+
Several methodologies of the Hawthorne experiments came under fire long after the results were published and accepted. In the experiments, the observed workers were in an improved setting compared to normal working conditions, and supervisors had a more positive attitude as observers rather than normal foremen. Extended rest periods, as well as pay increases and decreases, also played a role in the workers' performance. In one case, some of the subjects were dropped for a trend of decreasing outputs and replaced long after the experiment had begun. This may have had unknown effects on those who were there from the beginning. Any of these variables may have impacted the work performance independently of merely being observed. It is difficult to say to how large a degree these extraneous factors influenced the results, but these flaws in methodology have not been examined thoroughly.<ref> Parsons</ref>
  
Parsons redefines "the Hawthorne effect as the confounding that occurs if experimenters fail to realize how the consequences of subjects' performance affect what subjects do" [i.e. learning effects, both permanent skill improvement and feedback-enabled adjustments to suit current goals]. So he is saying it is not attention or warm regard from experimenters, but either a) actual change in rewards b) change in provision of feedback on performance. His key argument is that in the relay-assembly experiments, the subjects had access to the counters of their work rate, which they didn't previously know. <ref name=Parsons/>
+
== Results and Conclusions ==
  
It is notable however that he refuses to analyze the illumination experiments, which don't fit his analysis, on the grounds that they haven't been properly published and so he can't get at details, whereas he had extensive personal communication with Roethlisberger and Dickson.
+
The results of the experiment provide a few key points in terms of the different social and psychological factors that impact employee productivity.  
  
It's possible that the illumination experiments were explained by a [[longitudinal learning effect]]. But Mayo says it is to do with the fact that the workers felt better in the situation, because of the sympathy and interest of the observers. He does say that this experiment is about testing overall effect, not testing factors separately. He also discusses it not really as an experimenter effect but as a management effect: how management can make workers perform differently because they feel differently. A lot to do with feeling free, not feeling supervised but more in control as a group. The experimental manipulations were important in convincing the workers to feel this way: that conditions were really different. The experiment was repeated with similar effects on [[mica]] splitting workers.
+
The first deals with the over-reliance on the '''aptitude of individuals''' to determine job success. It is possible that strong physical and mental potential can have some determination of success in the work place, but '''social factors''' have more of an influence on work produced.  
  
When we refer to "the Hawthorne effect" we are pretty much referring to Mayo's interpretation in terms of workers' perceptions, but the data show strikingly [[continuous improvement]]. It seems quite a different interpretation might be possible: learning, expertise, reflection — are all processes independent of the experimental intervention? However the usual Mayo interpretation is certainly a real possible issue in designing studies in education and other areas, regardless of the truth of the original Hawthorne study.
+
The next point of interest describes the impact of '''informal organization on progress'''. The researchers in the experiment discovered a kind of work place society, as opposed to the mentality of isolated individuals previous studies had taken to workers. The more cohesive and friendly employees were to each other, and supervisors were to the workers, the higher the productivity and output from individuals.  
  
Recently the issue of "implicit social cognition" i.e. how much weight we actually give to what is implied by others' behavior towards us (as opposed to what they say e.g. flattery) has been discussed: this is considered to be an element of the Hawthorne effect as well.
+
The third discusses the '''norms of work-groups.''' The researchers saw the work-groups arrive at a group consensus about what a fair days amount of work is, and they set their efforts in accordance with this goal, resolving together to not work further even in the face of a financial incentive.  
  
== Evaluation ==
+
The final point suggests that the '''workplace is a social system made up of interdependent parts.''' The workplace, further, needs to be treated like a society in order to be successful.
  
The Hawthorne effect is notable from several levels of social awareness and cognition. At the top level, it seems clear that in some cases there is a large effect that experimenters did not anticipate, that is due to participants' reactions to the experiment itself. It only happens sometimes. So as a methodological heuristic (that you should always think about this issue) it is useful, but as an exact predictor of effects, it is not: often there is no Hawthorne effect of any kind. To understand when and why we will see a Hawthorne or experimenter effect, we need more detailed considerations.
+
The main idea taken away from this experiment is the impact of attention on those involved in an experiment.<ref> Henry A. Landsberger.  1958. ''Hawthorne Revisited.'' (Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.) Social Forces.</ref> When the workers realized they were being watched by the researchers, they modified their own behavior and acted in a different way than they may act when not being watched. When being watched, individuals behavior is improved to demonstrate a positive outward appearance. This conclusion has become a precedent for research experiments since this one, it has become customary to take the "Hawthorne effect" into consideration when creating and conducting any social or psychological experiment, as the results can be altered if this factor is not accounted for.<ref>Richard Gillespie. 1993. ''Manufacturing Knowledge: A History of the Hawthorne Experiments.'' (Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521456432). Retrieved August 12, 2007. </ref>
  
At a middle level <ref name=Adair/> says that the most important (though not the only) aspect of this is how the participants interpret the situation. Interviewing them (after the "experiment" part) would be the way to investigate this. This is important because factory workers, students, and most experimental participants are doing things at the request of the experimenter. What they do depends on what their personal goals are, how they understand the task requested, whether they want to please the experimenter and/or whether they see this task as impinging on other interests and goals they hold, what they think the experimenter really wants. Besides all those issues that determine their goals and intentions in the experiment, further aspects of how they understand the situation can be important by affecting what they believe about the effects of their actions. Thus the experimenter effect is really not one of interference, but of a possible difference in the meaning of the situation for participants and experimenter. Since all voluntary action (i.e. actions in most experiments) depends upon the actor's goals AND on their beliefs about the effects of their actions, differences in understanding of the situation can have big effects.
+
The Hawthorne experiments show that the work place is an area of [[socialization]] just like any other area where people congregate (schools, religious centers, and so forth). In the work groups of these experiments, the socialization was dependent upon the interactions amongst the workers and between their superiors and themselves. A strong, positive rapport led to higher productivity and happier workers. The workers also created their own norms of production, reflecting a kind of socialization where each worker realizes the limits of their coworkers and come to a mutual agreement concerning when they have produced enough work that day. These norms were rarely violated even in the face of financial incentive; the workers together acted as one part of a whole.
 
 
At the lowest level is the question of what the direct causal factors might be. These could include:
 
 
 
* Material ones that are intended by the experimenter
 
* Feedback that an experiment might make available to the participants
 
* Changes to goals, motivation, and beliefs about action effects induced by the experimental situation.
 
  
 
==Notes==
 
==Notes==
 
<references/>
 
<references/>
==References==
 
 
*Mayo, E. (1933) ''The human problems of an industrial civilization'' (New York: MacMillan) ch. 3.
 
*Roethlisberger, F. J. & Dickson, W. J. (1939) ''Management and the Worker'' (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press).
 
*Landsberger, Henry A. 1958. [http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-7732(195905)37%3A4%3C361%3AHR%3E2.0.CO%3B2-A ''Hawthorne Revisited.''] Ithaca, NY. Cornell University. Social Forces.
 
*Gillespie, Richard (1991) ''Manufacturing knowledge : a history of the Hawthorne experiments'' (Cambridge : Cambridge University Press)
 
* ''Was There a Hawthorne Effect?''  Stephen R. G. Jones, ''The American Journal of Sociology.'' '''98'''(3) (Nov., 1992), pp. 451-468, from the abstract "the main conclusion is that these data show slender to no evidence of the Hawthorne Effect"
 
* Franke, R.H. & Kaul, J.D. "The Hawthorne experiments: First statistical interpretation." American Sociological Review, 1978, 43, 623-643.
 
* [http://www.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/ Steve Draper], university professor of the UK.
 
  
==Further reading==
+
==Bibliography==
*G. Adair (1984) "The Hawthorne effect: A reconsideration of the methodological artifact" ''Journal of Appl. Psychology'' '''69''' (2), 334-345 [Reviews references to Hawthorne in the psychology methodology literature.]
 
*Clark, R. E. & Sugrue, B. M. (1991) "Research on instructional media, 1978-1988" in G. J. Anglin (ed.) ''Instructional technology: past, present, and future,'' ch.30, pp.327-343. Libraries unlimited: Englewood, Colorado.
 
*Gillespie, Richard, (1991) ''Manufacturing knowledge : a history of the Hawthorne experiments.'' Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
 
*Jastrow (1900) ''Fact and fable in psychology.'' Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
 
*Lovett, R. "Running on empty" ''New Scientist'' 20 March 2004 '''181''' no.2439 pp.42-45
 
*Leonard, K.L. and Masatu, M.C. "Outpatient process quality evaluation and the Hawthorne effect" ''Social Science and Medicine'' '''69''' no.9 pp.2330-2340
 
*Marsh, H.W. (1987) "Student's evaluations of university teaching: research findings, methodological issues, and directions for future research" ''Int. Journal of Educational Research'' '''11''' (3) pp.253-388.
 
*[[Elton Mayo]] (1933) ''The human problems of an industrial civilization'' (New York: MacMillan)
 
*Orne, M. T. (1973) "Communication by the total experimental situation: Why is it important, how it is evaluated, and its significance for the ecological validity of findings" in P. Pliner, L. Krames & T. Alloway (Eds.) ''Communication and affect'' pp.157-191. New York: Academic Press.
 
*H. M. Parsons (1974) "What happened at Hawthorne?" ''Science'' '''183''', 922-932 [A very detailed description, in a more accessible source, of some of the experiments; used to argue that the effect was due to feedback-promoted learning.]
 
*Fritz J. Roethlisberger & Dickson, W. J. (1939) ''Management and the Worker.'' Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
 
*Rosenthal, R. (1966) ''Experimenter effects in behavioral research'' (New York: Appleton).
 
*Rosenthal, R. & Jacobson, L. (1968, 1992) ''Pygmalion in the classroom: Teacher expectation and pupils' intellectual development''. Irvington publishers: New York.
 
*Rhem, J. (1999) "Pygmalion in the classroom" in ''The national teaching and learning forum'' 8 (2) pp. 1-4.
 
*Schön, D. A. (1983) ''The reflective practitioner: How professionals think in action'' (Temple Smith: London) (Basic books?)
 
*Shayer, M. (1992) "Problems and issues in intervention studies" in Demetriou, A., Shayer, M. & Efklides, A. (eds.) ''Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development: implications and applications for education'' ch. 6, pp.107-121. London: Routledge.
 
*Wall, P. D. (1999) ''Pain: the science of suffering.'' Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
 
*Zdep, S. M. & Irvine, S. H. (1970) "A reverse Hawthorne effect in educational evaluation." ''Journal of School Psychology'' '''8''', pp.89-95.
 
  
 +
* Adair, G. (1984) "The Hawthorne effect: A reconsideration of the methodological artifact" ''Journal of Appl. Psychology'' 69(2), 334-345 [Reviews references to Hawthorne in the psychology methodology literature.]
 +
* Dickson, W.J. and F.J. Roethlisberger. 1939. ''Management and the Worker.'' Harvard University Press. Cambridge, MA. ISBN 978-0415279871.
 +
* Gillespie, Richard. 1993. ''Manufacturing Knowledge: A History of the Hawthorne Experiments.'' Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521456432.
 +
* Jacobson, Lenore and Robert Rosenthal. 2003 (1968). ''Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher Expectation and Pupils' Intellectual Development.'' Crown House Publishing. ISBN 978-1904424062.
 +
*Jastrow, Joseph (1900) ''Fact and fable in psychology.'' Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
 +
* Landsberger, Henry A. 1958. [http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-7732(195905)37%3A4%3C361%3AHR%3E2.0.CO%3B2-A ''Hawthorne Revisited.''] Ithaca, NY. Cornell University. Social Forces.
 +
* Mayo, Elton. 2003 (1933). ''The Human Problems of an Industrial Civilization.'' London: Routledge. ISBN 978-0415279888.
 +
* Parsons, H. M. (1974) "What happened at Hawthorne?" ''Science'' 183: 922-932 [A very detailed description, in a more accessible source, of some of the experiments; used to argue that the effect was due to feedback-promoted learning.]
 +
* Roethlisberger, Fritz J. and W. J. Dickson, (1939) ''Management and the Worker.'' Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
 +
* Rosenthal, R. (1966) ''Experimenter effects in behavioral research.'' New York: Appleton.
 +
* Rosenthal, R. & L. Jacobson. (original 1968) 1992. ''Pygmalion in the classroom: Teacher expectation and pupils' intellectual development.'' New York: Irvington publishers.
 +
* Wall, Patrick D. (1999) ''Pain: the science of suffering.'' (Maps of the Mind) New York: Columbia University Press, (original 1999) 2000. ISBN 0231120060
  
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
*[http://www.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/hawth.html ''The Hawthorne, Pygmalion, placebo and other expectancy effects: some notes''], by Stephen W. Draper, Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow.  
+
All links retrieved December 11, 2017.
 +
*Stephen W. Draper [http://www.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/hawth.html ''The Hawthorne, Pygmalion, placebo and other expectancy effects: some notes''] ''Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow.''
  
 
{{Credits|Hawthorne_effect|140032114|}}
 
{{Credits|Hawthorne_effect|140032114|}}

Latest revision as of 15:03, 25 January 2023


The Hawthorne effect refers to a phenomenon which is thought to occur when people observed during a research study temporarily change their behavior or performance (this can also be referred to as demand characteristics). Others have broadened the definition to mean that people’s behavior and performance change following any new or increased attention. The Hawthorne studies have had a dramatic effect on management in organizations and understanding the impact of different factors in the workplace.

The purpose of the original experiments was to study the effect of lighting on workers’ productivity. When researchers found that productivity almost always increased after a change in illumination, no matter what the level of illumination was, a second set of experiments began, supervised by Harvard University professors Elton Mayo, Fritz Roethlisberger, and William J. Dickson.

They experimented on other types of changes in the working environment, using a study group of five young women. Again, no matter the change in conditions, the women nearly always produced more. The researchers reported that they had accidentally found a way to increase productivity. The effect was an important milestone in industrial and organizational psychology and in organizational behavior, bringing awareness to the impact of social factors and socialization in the workplace. In this way, management and researchers alike recognize that people are essentially social beings, and their relationships with others are highly significant in everything that they do.

However, some researchers have questioned the validity of the effect because of the experiments’ design and faulty interpretations. Thus, the Hawthorne effect refers not only to the findings relating to productivity, but also to the issue of people's behavior being changed by the experimental study itself, which confounds the factors under investigation.

Definition

The Hawthorne experiments sought to determine the motivating factors behind success and productivity in the work place. The hypothesis focused on economic factors being the biggest contributor to work place success.[1] However, the results suggested different factors were important.

Social factors of the employment itself were found to be the biggest motivator in job performance, mainly how much attention was being paid to individual employees. The Hawthorne effect is, essentially, the positive effect on a person or group's behavior from knowingly being watched. It is a positive effect because when a person suspects they are being observed, they are more prone to act at a higher level of efficiency than they normally act.[2] Another way to interpret this is the failure on the part of the experimenters to identify that their observation or experiment is motivating the subjects to act differently than they normally do; they act in a way that they think the scientists want them to act.[3]

History of the Hawthorne Experiments

Like the Hawthorne effect, the definition of the Hawthorne experiments also varies. The Hawthorne Works, located in Cicero, Illinois, just outside of Chicago, belonged to the Western Electric Company, and the studies were funded by the National Research Council of the National Academy of Sciences at the behest of General Electric, the largest manufacturer of light bulbs in the United States.[4] The purpose was to find the optimum level of lighting for productivity.

Most industrial and occupational psychology and organizational behavior textbooks refer to the illumination studies, and usually to the relay assembly test room experiments and the bank wiring room experiments. Only occasionally are the rest of the studies mentioned.[5]

Illumination Experiments

During two and a half years from 1924 to 1927, a series of illumination experiments using different study groups were conducted by the industrial engineers of Western Electric Company Works in Cicero; Illinois.[6]

  • Study 1a: In the first experiment, there was no control group. The researchers experimented on three different departments; all showed an increase of productivity, whether illumination increased or decreased.
  • Study 1b: A control group had no change in lighting, while the experimental group got a sequence of increasing light levels. Both groups substantially increased production, and there was no difference between the groups. This naturally piqued the researchers' curiosity.
  • Study 1c: The researchers decided to see what would happen if they decreased lighting. The control group had stable illumination; the other experienced a sequence of decreasing levels. Surprisingly, both groups steadily increased production until finally the light in experimental group became so low that they protested and production fell off.
  • Study 1d: This was conducted on two women only. Their production stayed constant under widely varying light levels. It was found that if the experimenter said bright was good, they said they preferred the light; the brighter they believed it to be, the more they liked it. The same was true when he said dimmer was good. If they were deceived about a change, they said they preferred it. Researchers concluded that their preference on lighting level was completely subjective - if they were told it was good, they believed it was good and preferred it, and vice versa.

At this point, researchers of Western Electric realized that something else besides lighting was affecting productivity. They suspected that the supervision of the researchers had some effect, so they ended the illumination experiments in 1927.

During 1927, a Harvard professor, Elton Mayo, along with his associates, joined the study as consultants. The experiments started again and lasted through to 1932. These experiments consisted of redesigning jobs, changing workday and workweek lengths, additional rest times, and studying the factor of wage changes.

Relay-Assembly Experiments

In the relay-assembly experiments, the researchers wanted to identify how other variables could affect productivity. They chose two women as test subjects and asked them to choose four other workers to join the test group. Together the women worked in a separate room over the course of five years (1927-1932) assembling telephone relays.[7]

Output was measured mechanically by counting how many finished relays each dropped down a chute. This measuring began in secret two weeks before moving the women to an experiment room and continued throughout the study. In the experiment room, they had a supervisor who discussed changes with them and at times used their suggestions. Then the researchers spent five years measuring how different variables impacted the group's and individuals' productivity. Some of the variables were:

  • changing the pay rules so that the group was paid for overall group production, not individual production
  • giving two five-minute breaks (after a discussion with them on the best length of time), and then changing to two ten-minute breaks (not their preference). Productivity increased, but when they received six five-minute rests, they disliked it and reduced output.
  • providing food during the breaks
  • shortening the day by 30 minutes (output went up); shortening it more (output per hour went up, but overall output decreased); returning to the earlier condition (where output peaked).

Changing a variable usually increased productivity, even if the variable was just a change back to the original condition. Researchers concluded that the workers worked harder because they thought that they were being monitored individually. However this can be interpreted as the natural processes of the human being to adapt to the environment without reference to the experiment.

Researchers hypothesized that choosing one's own coworkers, working as a group, being treated as special (as evidenced by working in a separate room), and having a sympathetic supervisor were the real reasons for the productivity increase. One interpretation, mainly due to Mayo, was that "the six individuals became a team and the team gave itself wholeheartedly and spontaneously to cooperation in the experiment." (There was a second relay assembly test room study whose results were not as significant as the first experiment.)

Bank-Wiring Room Experiments

The purpose of the bank wiring room experiments was to find out how payment incentives would affect group productivity. The surprising result was that they had no effect. Ironically, this contradicted the Hawthorne effect: although the workers were receiving special attention, it did not affect their behavior or productivity. However, the informal group dynamics studied were a new milestone in organizational behavior.[8]

The study was conducted by Mayo and W. Lloyd Warner between 1931 and 1932 on a group of 14 men who put together telephone switching equipment. The researchers found that although the workers were paid according to individual productivity, productivity did not go up because the men were afraid that the company would lower the base rate. The men also formed cliques, ostracized coworkers, and created a social hierarchy that was only partly related to the difference in their jobs. The cliques served to control group members and to manage bosses; when bosses asked questions, clique members gave the same responses, even if they were untrue.

Evaluation

The Hawthorne effect is notable from several perspectives of social awareness and cognition. It seems clear that in some cases there is a large effect that experimenters did not anticipate, that is due to participants' reactions to the experiment itself. So as a methodological heuristic, it is useful, but as an exact predictor of effects, it is not: it is difficult to ascertain the Hawthorne effect in every situation. To understand when and why we will see a Hawthorne or experimenter effect, we need more detailed considerations.

From another perspective, it is suggested that the most important (though not the only) aspect of this is how the participants interpret the situation.[9] This is important because factory workers, students, and most experimental participants are doing things at the request of the experimenter. What they do depends on what their personal goals are, how they understand the task requested, whether they want to please the experimenter and/or whether they see this task as impinging on other interests and goals they hold, what they think the experimenter really wants. Besides all those issues that determine their goals and intentions in the experiment, further aspects of how they understand the situation can be important by affecting what they believe about the effects of their actions.

Another issue is the question of what direct causal factors might be involved. These could include material ones that are intended by the experimenter, feedback that an experiment might make available to the participants, or changes to goals, motivation, and beliefs about action effects induced by the experimental situation.[10]

Criticism

Several methodologies of the Hawthorne experiments came under fire long after the results were published and accepted. In the experiments, the observed workers were in an improved setting compared to normal working conditions, and supervisors had a more positive attitude as observers rather than normal foremen. Extended rest periods, as well as pay increases and decreases, also played a role in the workers' performance. In one case, some of the subjects were dropped for a trend of decreasing outputs and replaced long after the experiment had begun. This may have had unknown effects on those who were there from the beginning. Any of these variables may have impacted the work performance independently of merely being observed. It is difficult to say to how large a degree these extraneous factors influenced the results, but these flaws in methodology have not been examined thoroughly.[11]

Results and Conclusions

The results of the experiment provide a few key points in terms of the different social and psychological factors that impact employee productivity.

The first deals with the over-reliance on the aptitude of individuals to determine job success. It is possible that strong physical and mental potential can have some determination of success in the work place, but social factors have more of an influence on work produced.

The next point of interest describes the impact of informal organization on progress. The researchers in the experiment discovered a kind of work place society, as opposed to the mentality of isolated individuals previous studies had taken to workers. The more cohesive and friendly employees were to each other, and supervisors were to the workers, the higher the productivity and output from individuals.

The third discusses the norms of work-groups. The researchers saw the work-groups arrive at a group consensus about what a fair days amount of work is, and they set their efforts in accordance with this goal, resolving together to not work further even in the face of a financial incentive.

The final point suggests that the workplace is a social system made up of interdependent parts. The workplace, further, needs to be treated like a society in order to be successful.

The main idea taken away from this experiment is the impact of attention on those involved in an experiment.[12] When the workers realized they were being watched by the researchers, they modified their own behavior and acted in a different way than they may act when not being watched. When being watched, individuals behavior is improved to demonstrate a positive outward appearance. This conclusion has become a precedent for research experiments since this one, it has become customary to take the "Hawthorne effect" into consideration when creating and conducting any social or psychological experiment, as the results can be altered if this factor is not accounted for.[13]

The Hawthorne experiments show that the work place is an area of socialization just like any other area where people congregate (schools, religious centers, and so forth). In the work groups of these experiments, the socialization was dependent upon the interactions amongst the workers and between their superiors and themselves. A strong, positive rapport led to higher productivity and happier workers. The workers also created their own norms of production, reflecting a kind of socialization where each worker realizes the limits of their coworkers and come to a mutual agreement concerning when they have produced enough work that day. These norms were rarely violated even in the face of financial incentive; the workers together acted as one part of a whole.

Notes

  1. Stephen Draper. 2006.Finding The Hawthorne, Pygmalion, Placebo and Other Effects of Expectation. University of Glasgow. Retrieved July 31, 2007.
  2. Lester Breslow and John Last. 2002. Encyclopedia of Public Health. Macmillan Reference. ISBN 0028653548. Retrieved August 14, 2007.
  3. H. M. Parsons, 1974. What Happened at Hawthorne? Science Journal 183 (4128)(March 8, 1974): 922-932. Retrieved July 31, 2007.
  4. Edwin Lee Makamson. 2000. The Hawthorne Experiments: Management Takes A New Direction. Management Guru. Retrieved July 31, 2007.
  5. R. Olson, J. Verley, L. Santos, and C. Salas. What We Teach Students About the Hawthorne Studies: A Review of Content Within a Sample of Introductory I-O and OB Textbooks Santa Clara Univeristy.
  6. W.J. Dickson and F.J. Roethlisberger, 1939. Management and the Worker. (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0415279871). Retrieved July 31, 2007.
  7. Austin Weber. 2002. The Hawthorne Works. Assembly Magazine. Retrieved August 5, 2007.
  8. Ibid.
  9. John Adair, Donald Sharpe, and Cam-Loi Huynh. 1989. Hawthorne Control Procedures in Educational Experiments: A Reconsideration of Their Use and Effectiveness Review of Educational Research 59 (2)(Summer, 1989): 215-228.
  10. Donald Campbell, and Julian Stanley. 1963. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research. (Houghton Mifflin Company. ISBN 978-0395307878). Retrieved August 4, 2007.
  11. Parsons
  12. Henry A. Landsberger. 1958. Hawthorne Revisited. (Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.) Social Forces.
  13. Richard Gillespie. 1993. Manufacturing Knowledge: A History of the Hawthorne Experiments. (Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521456432). Retrieved August 12, 2007.

Bibliography

  • Adair, G. (1984) "The Hawthorne effect: A reconsideration of the methodological artifact" Journal of Appl. Psychology 69(2), 334-345 [Reviews references to Hawthorne in the psychology methodology literature.]
  • Dickson, W.J. and F.J. Roethlisberger. 1939. Management and the Worker. Harvard University Press. Cambridge, MA. ISBN 978-0415279871.
  • Gillespie, Richard. 1993. Manufacturing Knowledge: A History of the Hawthorne Experiments. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521456432.
  • Jacobson, Lenore and Robert Rosenthal. 2003 (1968). Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher Expectation and Pupils' Intellectual Development. Crown House Publishing. ISBN 978-1904424062.
  • Jastrow, Joseph (1900) Fact and fable in psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Landsberger, Henry A. 1958. Hawthorne Revisited. Ithaca, NY. Cornell University. Social Forces.
  • Mayo, Elton. 2003 (1933). The Human Problems of an Industrial Civilization. London: Routledge. ISBN 978-0415279888.
  • Parsons, H. M. (1974) "What happened at Hawthorne?" Science 183: 922-932 [A very detailed description, in a more accessible source, of some of the experiments; used to argue that the effect was due to feedback-promoted learning.]
  • Roethlisberger, Fritz J. and W. J. Dickson, (1939) Management and the Worker. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Rosenthal, R. (1966) Experimenter effects in behavioral research. New York: Appleton.
  • Rosenthal, R. & L. Jacobson. (original 1968) 1992. Pygmalion in the classroom: Teacher expectation and pupils' intellectual development. New York: Irvington publishers.
  • Wall, Patrick D. (1999) Pain: the science of suffering. (Maps of the Mind) New York: Columbia University Press, (original 1999) 2000. ISBN 0231120060

External links

All links retrieved December 11, 2017.

Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.