Difference between revisions of "Imprinting (psychology)" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
Line 5: Line 5:
  
 
'''Imprinting''' is the term used in [[psychology]] and [[ethology]] to describe any kind of phase-sensitive learning (learning occurring at a particular age or a particular life stage) that is rapid and apparently independent of the consequences of behavior.  It typically involves an animal or person learning the characteristics of some stimulus, which is therefore said to be "imprinted" onto the subject. Imprinting is a term which describes a learning process during sensitive periods of life by which animals learn their preferences toward and a particular object or class of objects.
 
'''Imprinting''' is the term used in [[psychology]] and [[ethology]] to describe any kind of phase-sensitive learning (learning occurring at a particular age or a particular life stage) that is rapid and apparently independent of the consequences of behavior.  It typically involves an animal or person learning the characteristics of some stimulus, which is therefore said to be "imprinted" onto the subject. Imprinting is a term which describes a learning process during sensitive periods of life by which animals learn their preferences toward and a particular object or class of objects.
 +
 +
==Prevalence of Imprinting==
 +
 +
Behavioral imprinting is a natural survival instinct. Theoretically speaking, an infant of any animal kingdom should have the ability to recognize their parent in order to guarantee their well-being during a dangerous event. This makes imprinting both a reliable and necessary bond between the parent and their newborn. Imprinting is most obvious in nidifugous birds. Generally speaking, mammals aren't known to imprint the same way that nidifugous birds do, but there is a strong mother-child bond that develops through lengthier processes.
  
 
==Filial imprinting==
 
==Filial imprinting==
  
The best known form of imprinting is '''filial imprinting''', in which a young animal learns the characteristics of its parent.  It is most obvious in [[nidifugous]] birds, who imprint on their parents and then follow them around.   
+
A widely researched form of imprinting is known as '''filial imprinting'''. Filial imprinit occurs when a young animal learns the characteristics of its parent.  It is most obvious in [[nidifugous]] birds, who imprint on their parents and then follow them around.   
  
 
'''Filial imprinting''' was first reported in domestic [[chicken]]s, by the [[19th century]] amateur biologist [[Douglas Spalding]].  It was rediscovered by the early ethologist [[Oskar Heinroth]], and studied extensively and popularized by his disciple [[Konrad Lorenz]] working with [[Greylag Goose|greylag geese]].  Lorenz demonstrated how incubator-hatched geese would imprint on the first suitable moving stimulus they saw within what he called a "[[critical period]]" of about 36 hours shortly after hatching.   
 
'''Filial imprinting''' was first reported in domestic [[chicken]]s, by the [[19th century]] amateur biologist [[Douglas Spalding]].  It was rediscovered by the early ethologist [[Oskar Heinroth]], and studied extensively and popularized by his disciple [[Konrad Lorenz]] working with [[Greylag Goose|greylag geese]].  Lorenz demonstrated how incubator-hatched geese would imprint on the first suitable moving stimulus they saw within what he called a "[[critical period]]" of about 36 hours shortly after hatching.   
Line 28: Line 32:
 
''Reverse'' sexual imprinting is also seen: when two people live in close domestic proximity during the first few years in the life of either one, both are desensitized to later close [[sexual attraction]] and [[bonding]]. This phenomenon, known as the '''Westermarck effect''', was discovered by [[anthropologist]] [[Edvard Westermarck]].  The Westermarck effect has since been observed in many places and cultures, including in the [[Israel]]i [[kibbutz]] system, and the [[Shim-pua marriage]] customs of [[Taiwan]], as well as in biological-related families.
 
''Reverse'' sexual imprinting is also seen: when two people live in close domestic proximity during the first few years in the life of either one, both are desensitized to later close [[sexual attraction]] and [[bonding]]. This phenomenon, known as the '''Westermarck effect''', was discovered by [[anthropologist]] [[Edvard Westermarck]].  The Westermarck effect has since been observed in many places and cultures, including in the [[Israel]]i [[kibbutz]] system, and the [[Shim-pua marriage]] customs of [[Taiwan]], as well as in biological-related families.
  
In the case of the Israeli [[kibbutz]] farms, these children grew up in a common children's house, away from their parents. They spent the entire day and night together. This resulted in a generation that was not interested in the opposite sex within their class. It is an extreme example of grouping since the adults were also removed from the environment.
+
When this does not occur, for example where a brother and sister are  brought up not knowing about one another, they may find one another highly sexually attractive when they meet as adults: a phenomenon known as [[genetic sexual attraction]]. This observation is consistent with the theory that the Westermarck effect evolved to suppress [[inbreeding]].
  
When this does not occur, for example where a brother and sister are  brought up not knowing about one another, they may find one another highly sexually attractive when they meet as adults: a phenomenon known as [[genetic sexual attraction]]. This observation is consistent with the theory that the Westermarck effect evolved to suppress [[inbreeding]].
+
==Recent Research==
 +
 
 +
Recent research contends that the tradition of Lorenz's imprinting may be somewhat incomplete. Studying his subjects in their natural environment, it is argued that Lorenz lacked the objectivity that can be facilitated in a laboratory. The experimental approach of modern reseach has allowed for understanding of the  
  
 
==References==
 
==References==

Revision as of 05:54, 25 August 2006



Imprinting is the term used in psychology and ethology to describe any kind of phase-sensitive learning (learning occurring at a particular age or a particular life stage) that is rapid and apparently independent of the consequences of behavior. It typically involves an animal or person learning the characteristics of some stimulus, which is therefore said to be "imprinted" onto the subject. Imprinting is a term which describes a learning process during sensitive periods of life by which animals learn their preferences toward and a particular object or class of objects.

Prevalence of Imprinting

Behavioral imprinting is a natural survival instinct. Theoretically speaking, an infant of any animal kingdom should have the ability to recognize their parent in order to guarantee their well-being during a dangerous event. This makes imprinting both a reliable and necessary bond between the parent and their newborn. Imprinting is most obvious in nidifugous birds. Generally speaking, mammals aren't known to imprint the same way that nidifugous birds do, but there is a strong mother-child bond that develops through lengthier processes.

Filial imprinting

A widely researched form of imprinting is known as filial imprinting. Filial imprinit occurs when a young animal learns the characteristics of its parent. It is most obvious in nidifugous birds, who imprint on their parents and then follow them around.

Filial imprinting was first reported in domestic chickens, by the 19th century amateur biologist Douglas Spalding. It was rediscovered by the early ethologist Oskar Heinroth, and studied extensively and popularized by his disciple Konrad Lorenz working with greylag geese. Lorenz demonstrated how incubator-hatched geese would imprint on the first suitable moving stimulus they saw within what he called a "critical period" of about 36 hours shortly after hatching.

Most famously, the goslings would imprint on Lorenz himself because of this, he is often depicted being followed by a gaggle of geese who had imprinted on him. Filial imprinting is not restricted to animals that are able to follow their parents, however; in child development the term is used to refer to the process by which a baby learns who its mother and father are. The process is recognized as beginning in the womb, when the unborn baby starts to recognize its parents' voices.

The Italian hang-glider pilot Angelo d'Arrigo extended this technique. D'Arrigo noted that the flight of a non-motorised hang-glider is very similar to the flight patterns of migratory birds: both use updrafts of hot air (thermal currents) to gain altitude which then permits soaring flight over distance. He used this fact to enable the re-introduction into the wild of threatened species of raptors. Birds which are hatched in captivity have no mentor birds to teach them their traditional migratory routes. D'Arrigo had one solution to this problem. The chicks hatched under the wing of his glider, and imprinted on him. Subsequently, he taught the fledglings to fly and to hunt. The young birds followed him not only on the ground (as with Lorenz) but also in the air as he took the path of various migratory routes. He flew across the Sahara and over the Mediterranean Sea to Sicily with eagles, from Siberia to Iran (5,500 km) with a flock of Siberian cranes, and over Everest with Nepalese eagles. In 2006, he worked with a condor in South America.

In a similar project, orphaned Canada Geese were trained to their normal migration route by the Canadian ultralight enthusiast Bill Lishman, as shown in the fact based movie drama Fly Away Home.

Sexual imprinting

Sexual imprinting is the process by which a young animal learns the characteristics of a desirable mate. For example, male zebra finches appear to prefer mates with the appearance of the female bird that rears them, rather than mates of their own type (Immelmann, 1972).

Sexual imprinting on inanimate objects is a popular theory concerning the development of sexual fetishism.

Westermarck effect

Reverse sexual imprinting is also seen: when two people live in close domestic proximity during the first few years in the life of either one, both are desensitized to later close sexual attraction and bonding. This phenomenon, known as the Westermarck effect, was discovered by anthropologist Edvard Westermarck. The Westermarck effect has since been observed in many places and cultures, including in the Israeli kibbutz system, and the Shim-pua marriage customs of Taiwan, as well as in biological-related families.

When this does not occur, for example where a brother and sister are brought up not knowing about one another, they may find one another highly sexually attractive when they meet as adults: a phenomenon known as genetic sexual attraction. This observation is consistent with the theory that the Westermarck effect evolved to suppress inbreeding.

Recent Research

Recent research contends that the tradition of Lorenz's imprinting may be somewhat incomplete. Studying his subjects in their natural environment, it is argued that Lorenz lacked the objectivity that can be facilitated in a laboratory. The experimental approach of modern reseach has allowed for understanding of the

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Westermarck, E. A. The history of human marriage, 5th edn. London: Macmillan, 1921.
  • Immelmann, K. (1972) Sexual and other long-term aspects of imprinting in birds and other species. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 4, 147–174.

External links

  • Cardoso, SH and Sabbatini, RME. Learning who is your mother. The behavior of imprinting. Brain & Mind Magazine.
  • Nancy T. Burley, a researcher into imprinting in zebra finches
  • Debra Lieberman, John Tooby and Leda Cosmides. "Does morality have a biological basis? An empirical test of the factors governing moral sentiments relating to incest." Accepted for publication in Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B November 2002. Available online at Citeseer
  • Angelo d'Arrigo personal website


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.