Difference between revisions of "Lilac chaser" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
(Copied from wikipedia)
 
(13 intermediate revisions by 6 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Claimed}}
+
{{Images OK}}{{Submitted}}{{Approved}}{{Paid}}{{Copyedited}}
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Politics and social sciences]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 
[[Category:Psychology]]
 +
[[Category:Illusion]]
 +
[[Image:Lilac-Chaser.gif|right|thumb|380px|Stare at the center cross for at least 30 seconds to experience the three phenomena of the illusion.]]
  
 +
'''Lilac chaser''' is a [[visual illusion]], also known as the '''Pac-Man illusion''', that spread across the [[Internet]] in 2005. Lilac chaser is very similar to [[phi phenomenon]], and consists of 12 [[lilac (color)|lilac]] (or pink or magenta-like), blurred disks arranged in a circle (like the numbers on a [[clock]]), around a small, black, central cross on a gray background.
 +
{{toc}}
 +
The apparent movement of the lilac chaser and similar phi phenomena is not fully understood, but it has led to significant research into visual perception. In addition to providing ways to research human abilities, illusions like the lilac chaser intrigue and entertain people, reflecting our cognitive curiosity and endless appreciation for [[creativity]] that is part of human nature.
  
[[Image:Lilac-Chaser.gif|right|thumb|400px|Demonstration of the illusion. Look at the central cross. Three effects will appear in order: First, the gap may appear to move clockwise, or a single disk adjacent to the gap may appear to move anticlockwise. This quickly stabilizes to become a gap moving clockwise. Second, the moving gap appears as a green disk. Third, the moving green disk will appear to wipe out the lilac disks, until only the green disk is visible. This picture however is not genuine: The pink discs don't just disappear - If you look at an individual frame of the animation and zoom in, you can clearly see that there is a slightly blue disc replacing what should be just a gap, possibly to reinforce the illusion.]]
+
==Discovery==
 +
The illusion was created by [[Jeremy Hinton]] some time before 2005. Hinton stumbled across the configuration while devising stimuli for visual motion experiments. In one version of a program to move a disk around a central point, he mistakenly omitted to erase the preceding disk, which created the appearance of a moving gap. Upon noticing the moving green-disk afterimage, he adjusted foreground and background colors, number of disks, and timing in order to optimize the effect. Later, Hinton blurred the disks, allowing them to disappear when a viewer looks steadily at the central cross. Hinton entered the illusion in the [[ECVP]] Visual Illusion Contest, but was disqualified for lack of registration. He then approached [[Michael Bach]], who placed it on his web page of illusions<ref>Michael Bach, [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/col_lilacChaser/index.html Web Page of illusions], Michaelbach.com, 2008. Retrieved January 7, 2007.</ref> and named it. The illusion quickly spread across the internet.
  
'''Lilac chaser''' is a [[visual illusion]], also known as the '''[[pacman]] illusion'''. It consists of 12 lilac (or pink or magenta-like), blurred disks arranged in a circle (like the numbers on a [[clock]]), around a small, black, central cross on a grey background. One of the disks disappears briefly (for about 0.1 second), then the next (about 0.125 second later), and the next, and so on, in a clockwise direction. When one stares at the cross for about 20 seconds or so, one first sees a gap running around the circle of lilac disks, then a green disk running around the circle of lilac disks, then a green disk running around on the grey background, the lilac disks having disappeared or to have been erased by the green disk.
+
==Description==
 
+
Lilac chaser consists of a group of 12 lilac, blurred disks arranged around a small black cross. Each disk disappears briefly in clockwise order. When staring at the central cross, a viewer first sees a gap rotating around the circle, then a green disk that rotates around the circle, and finally, the disappearance of the lilac disks themselves.
An interactive version of the illusion may be found [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/col_lilacChaser/index.html here]. This version allows viewers to adjust the [[color]], [[saturation (color theory)|saturation]], and timing of the disks.
 
 
 
The illusion spread around the [[Internet]] in 2005.
 
 
 
==History==
 
The illusion was devised by [[Jeremy Hinton]] some time before 2005. He stumbled across the configuration while devising stimuli for visual motion experiments. In one version of a program to move a disk around a central point, he mistakenly omitted to erase the preceding disk, giving the moving gap. On noticing the moving green-disk afterimage, he adjusted foreground and background colors, number of disks, and timing to optimize the effect.
 
 
 
In 2005 Hinton blurred the disks, allowing them to disappear when a viewer looks steadily at the central cross. Hinton entered the illusion in the [[ECVP]] Visual Illusion Contest, but was disqualified from his not being registered for that year's conference. He then approached [[Michael Bach]], who placed it on his [http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/col_lilacChaser/index.html web page of illusions], and named it. Then the illusion was duplicated on a web page for Mark Levinson's Design Services [http://www.patmedia.net/marklevinson/cool/cool_illusion.html]. Someone noticed this version and promoted it via e-mail or via an electronic bulletin board. Others did the same, spreading the illusion across the internet. A web search for '''rotating pink dot''' (which is the illusion's most widespread nickname) in November 2005 yielded more than 800 replications.
 
  
 
==Explanation==
 
==Explanation==
 +
The lilac chaser illusion combines three simple, well-known effects:
  
The lilac chaser illusion combines three simple, and well-known effects:
+
The first effect (the moving gap) is due to the [[phi phenomenon]]. The moving gap creates a sense of [[apparent motion]], similar to the way we perceive a series of still pictures as a moving image in [[film]].
 
 
First, when a visual event occurs briefly at one place in the visual field, and then a similar event occurs at an adjacent place in the same visual field, we see [[motion_perception|movement]] from the first place to the second. This is called [[apparent_motion|apparent movement]] or [[beta movement]] (see also [[motion perception]]), because no actual movement has occurred. The visual events are the disappearances of the lilac disks. Initially, we see something moving around the circle of lilac disks, as though something dark has passed over them, the visual event being a disappearance. Apparent movement is the basis of moving [[neon sign]]s, [[film]], and [[video]]. We see movement because such displays stimulate receptors (called [[Reichardt detector]]s) in our [[visual_cortex|brain]]s that encode movement.
 
 
 
Second, when a lilac stimulus that is presented to a particular region of the visual field for a long time (say 10 seconds or so) disappears, a green [[afterimage]] will appear. The afterimage lasts only a short time, and in this case is effaced by the reappearance of the lilac stimulus. The afterimage is a simple consequence of [[adaptation]] of the [[Rod cell|rods]] and [[cone cell|cones]] of the [[retina]].  Colour and brightness are encoded by the ratios of activities in three types of cones (and also with the rods). The cones stimulated by lilac get "tired". When the stimulus disappears, the tiredness of some of the cones means that the ratios evoked by the grey background are the same as if a green stimulus had been presented. Adaptation of rods and cones begins immediately they are stimulated, so afterimages also start to grow. We normally do not notice them because we [[Fixational_eye_movement|move our eyes]] about three times a second, so the image of a stimulus constantly falls on new, "fresh", unadapted rods and cones. In this case, we keep our eyes still, so the afterimages grow and are revealed when the stimulus disappears.
 
 
 
Third, when a blurry stimulus is presented to a region of the visual field away from where we are fixating, and we keep our eyes still, that stimulus will disappear even though it is still physically presented. This is called [[Troxler's fading]]. It occurs because although our eyes move a little when we are fixating a point, away from that point (in ''[[peripheral vision]]'') the movements are not large enough to shift the lilac disks to onto new neurons of the visual system. Their afterimages essentially cancel the original images, so that all one sees of the lilac disks is grey.
 
 
 
These effects combine to yield the remarkable sight of a green spot running around in a circle on a grey background when only stationary, flashing lilac spots have been presented.
 
 
 
==Psychophysics==
 
 
 
By December 2005, no systematic study of the stimulus properties of the illusion had been published. Hinton optimized the conditions for all three aspects of the illusion before releasing it. He also noted that the color of the green disk could be outside the color gamut of the monitor on which it was created. Michael Bach's version of the illusion allows viewers to adjust some aspects of the illusion. It is simple to confirm that the illusion occurs with other colors, and that Troxler fading is enhanced by reducing the saturation of the disks.
 
 
 
==Other effects from lilac chaser==
 
 
 
It is not necessary to fixate the black cross for the effects to occur. As long as the eyes are held steadily on any point of the figure (e.g., the centre of the three o'clock disk), they will occur.
 
 
 
If instead of fixating the black cross, one follows the moving gap with one's eyes, one will see only a moving gap and 12 lilac disks rather than a single green disk. This is because the green disk arises as an afterimage, requiring the eyes to be held steadily to occur, and the disappearance of the lilac disks is from Troxler's fading, also requiring the eyes to be held steadily to occur. Moving the eyes prevents the formation of afterimages and the occurrence of Troxler's fading.
 
 
 
If after looking at the effect for 30 seconds or so, one moves one's eyes elsewhere (e.g., to another point on the figure or to a blank sheet of white paper), one will see a stationary ring of 12 green disks that will fade in about 20% of the time spent looking at the effect. These green disks are the afterimages of the 12 lilac disks.
 
 
 
==Other illusions involving color==
 
*[[Same color]]
 
  
==Bibliography==
+
The moving gap becomes a rotating green disk because of [[afterimage]]s. When the [[retina]] is stimulated with a certain color, the [[cone cell|cones]] of the eye react to a loss of stimulus by activating the [[complimentary]] color of the stimulus. Adaptation of rods and cones begins immediately when they are stimulated, when afterimages also start to grow. We normally do not notice them because we [[Fixational eye movement|move our eyes]] about three times a second. In lilac chaser, we keep our eyes still, so the afterimages grow and are revealed as the stimulus disappears.
  
*[http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/col_lilacChaser/index.html "Michael Bach's explanation]
+
The blurred lilac disks disappear because of a phenomena known as [[Troxler's fading]]. When a blurry stimulus is presented to a region of the visual field away from where we are fixating, and we keep our eyes still, that stimulus will disappear even though it is still physically present. This occurs because, although our eyes move slightly when we are fixating at a point, in [[peripheral vision]] these movements are not large enough to shift the lilac disks to new neurons of the visual system. Their afterimages essentially cancel out the original images, and all one sees of the lilac disks is gray, except for the gap where the green afterimage appears. In fact, it is not necessary to fixate on the cross for the effect; any stationary fixation will work.
*[http://electroneubio.secyt.gov.ar/non-optic_visual_intonation.htm "Electroneurobiology article"]. The [[ontological]] nature of the color afterimages have been analyzed in this article, "A visual yet non-optical subjective intonation", by Mariela Szirko.
 
  
==See also==
+
These effects combine to yield the remarkable sight of a green spot running around in a circle on a gray background where only stationary, flashing lilac spots occur. Occasionally it may seem as though the green afterimage eats up the lilac disks, giving the illusion its alternative, [[Pac-Man]] name.
  
*[[Subjectivism]]
+
==Applications==
 +
Lilac chaser achieved a sort of cult-status on the [[Internet]]. Like many other similar illusions, the study of the perceptual effects of the illusion can be useful to scientists in the study of how the brain perceives and interprets visual information.
  
 +
==Notes==
 +
<references/>
  
 +
==References==
 +
* Blue, Ronald C. 2005. [http://electroneubio.secyt.gov.ar/non-optic_visual_intonation.htm A visual yet non-optical subjective intonation]. 13 (3), p. 299 - 300. Electroneurobiología, 2005. Retrieved December 21, 2007.
  
 +
==External links==
 +
All links retrieved October 25, 2022.
  
 +
*[http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/col-lilacChaser/index.html Hinton's “Lilac Chaser”]
 +
*[http://electroneubio.secyt.gov.ar/non-optic_visual_intonation.htm "Electroneurobiology article"] – The ontological nature of the color afterimages has been analyzed in this article, "A visual yet non-optical subjective intonation," by Mariela Szirko.
  
{{Credits|Lilac_chaser|119481722|}}
+
{{Credits|Lilac_chaser|156710438|}}

Latest revision as of 01:40, 26 October 2022

Stare at the center cross for at least 30 seconds to experience the three phenomena of the illusion.

Lilac chaser is a visual illusion, also known as the Pac-Man illusion, that spread across the Internet in 2005. Lilac chaser is very similar to phi phenomenon, and consists of 12 lilac (or pink or magenta-like), blurred disks arranged in a circle (like the numbers on a clock), around a small, black, central cross on a gray background.

The apparent movement of the lilac chaser and similar phi phenomena is not fully understood, but it has led to significant research into visual perception. In addition to providing ways to research human abilities, illusions like the lilac chaser intrigue and entertain people, reflecting our cognitive curiosity and endless appreciation for creativity that is part of human nature.

Discovery

The illusion was created by Jeremy Hinton some time before 2005. Hinton stumbled across the configuration while devising stimuli for visual motion experiments. In one version of a program to move a disk around a central point, he mistakenly omitted to erase the preceding disk, which created the appearance of a moving gap. Upon noticing the moving green-disk afterimage, he adjusted foreground and background colors, number of disks, and timing in order to optimize the effect. Later, Hinton blurred the disks, allowing them to disappear when a viewer looks steadily at the central cross. Hinton entered the illusion in the ECVP Visual Illusion Contest, but was disqualified for lack of registration. He then approached Michael Bach, who placed it on his web page of illusions[1] and named it. The illusion quickly spread across the internet.

Description

Lilac chaser consists of a group of 12 lilac, blurred disks arranged around a small black cross. Each disk disappears briefly in clockwise order. When staring at the central cross, a viewer first sees a gap rotating around the circle, then a green disk that rotates around the circle, and finally, the disappearance of the lilac disks themselves.

Explanation

The lilac chaser illusion combines three simple, well-known effects:

The first effect (the moving gap) is due to the phi phenomenon. The moving gap creates a sense of apparent motion, similar to the way we perceive a series of still pictures as a moving image in film.

The moving gap becomes a rotating green disk because of afterimages. When the retina is stimulated with a certain color, the cones of the eye react to a loss of stimulus by activating the complimentary color of the stimulus. Adaptation of rods and cones begins immediately when they are stimulated, when afterimages also start to grow. We normally do not notice them because we move our eyes about three times a second. In lilac chaser, we keep our eyes still, so the afterimages grow and are revealed as the stimulus disappears.

The blurred lilac disks disappear because of a phenomena known as Troxler's fading. When a blurry stimulus is presented to a region of the visual field away from where we are fixating, and we keep our eyes still, that stimulus will disappear even though it is still physically present. This occurs because, although our eyes move slightly when we are fixating at a point, in peripheral vision these movements are not large enough to shift the lilac disks to new neurons of the visual system. Their afterimages essentially cancel out the original images, and all one sees of the lilac disks is gray, except for the gap where the green afterimage appears. In fact, it is not necessary to fixate on the cross for the effect; any stationary fixation will work.

These effects combine to yield the remarkable sight of a green spot running around in a circle on a gray background where only stationary, flashing lilac spots occur. Occasionally it may seem as though the green afterimage eats up the lilac disks, giving the illusion its alternative, Pac-Man name.

Applications

Lilac chaser achieved a sort of cult-status on the Internet. Like many other similar illusions, the study of the perceptual effects of the illusion can be useful to scientists in the study of how the brain perceives and interprets visual information.

Notes

  1. Michael Bach, Web Page of illusions, Michaelbach.com, 2008. Retrieved January 7, 2007.

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

External links

All links retrieved October 25, 2022.

Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.