Encyclopedia, Difference between revisions of "Konrad Lorenz" - New World

From New World Encyclopedia
Line 5: Line 5:
  
 
{{epname}}
 
{{epname}}
 +
{{Infobox Scientist
 +
| name = Konrad Lorenz
 +
| image = Konrad lorenz-rusky lagr 1944-fotka.jpg
 +
| birth_date = November 7, 1903
 +
| birth_place = [[Image:Austria-Hungary flag 1869-1918.svg|25px]]  [[Vienna]], [[Austria-Hungary]]
 +
| death_date = February 27, 1989
 +
| death_place = {{flagicon|AUT}} [[Vienna]], [[Austria]]
 +
| residence = [[Austria]], [[Germany]]
 +
| nationality = [[Austria]]n
 +
| field = [[Ethology]]|
 +
| known_for = [[Imprinting (psychology)|Imprinting]] 
 +
| prizes = [[Image:Nobel.svg|20px]] [[Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine]] (1973)
 +
| field = [[zoology|Zoologist]], [[ethology|ethologist]]
 +
| alma_mater = University of Vienna
 +
| religion =
 +
| footnotes =
 +
}}
 +
'''Konrad Zacharias Lorenz''' (November 7, 1903 – February 27, 1989) was an [[Austria|Austrian]] [[zoology|zoologist]], [[animal psychology|animal psychologist]], and [[ornithologist]]. He is considered one of the founders of modern [[ethology]]. Lorenz studied [[instinct|instinctive behavior]] in [[animal]]s, especially in [[Greylag Goose|greylag geese]] and [[jackdaw]]s. Working with geese, he re-discovered the principle of [[imprinting (psychology)|imprinting]] (originally described by [[Douglas Spalding]] in the [[19th century]]) in the behavior of [[nidifugous]] [[bird]]s.
  
'''Konrad Zacharias Lorenz''' ([[November 7]], [[1903]] in [[Vienna]] – [[February 27]], [[1989]] in [[Vienna]]) was an [[Austria|Austrian]] [[zoology|zoologist]], [[animal psychology|animal psychologist]], and [[ornithologist]]. He is considered one of the founders of modern [[ethology]]. Lorenz studied [[instinct|instinctive behavior]] in [[animal]]s, especially in [[Greylag Goose|greylag geese]] and [[jackdaw]]s. Working with geese, he re-discovered the principle of [[imprinting (psychology)|imprinting]] (originally described by [[Douglas Spalding]] in the [[19th century]]) in the behavior of [[nidifugous]] [[bird]]s.  
+
== Life ==
 +
'''Konrad Zacharias Lorenz''' was born in Altenberg, near [[Vienna]], [[Austria]], to Adolf Lorenz and Emma Lecher Lorenz, both medical doctors. Always fascinated with animals, he kept a menagerie at home. His father insisted that he became a physician. At the request of his father, Konrad Lorenz began a premedical curriculum in 1922 at [[Columbia University]]. He returned to Vienna in 1923 to continue his studies at the [[University of Vienna]] until 1928. Konrad Lorenz received his medical degree in 1928, from the University of Vienna. A year before that, he married Margarethe Gebhart, also a doctor. Then studying at Ferdinand Hochstetter's Anatomical Institute at the University, he received his doctorate in [[zoology]], in 1933. During his early period, he set up many of his hypotheses on animal behavior, such as [[imprinting (psychology)|imprinting]], ''innate releasing mechanism'', and ''fixed action patterns''. After serving as an assistant in the Anatomical Institute, Lorenz was a ''Privatdozent'' (unpaid instructor) at the University of Vienna from 1937 to 1940.
  
= Life =
+
During 1930s, Lorenz established the major theoretical foundations of classical ethology. His basic insight, shared with predecessors and teachers, was that some instinctive behavior patterns were fixed in form and just as characteristic of species as organs. Lorenz planned to analyse instinctive behavior using comparative techniques. Lorenz joined the [[National Socialist German Workers Party|Nazi Party]] in 1938 and accepted a university chair under the [[Nazi Germany|Nazi regime]]. In his application for membership to the Nazi-party NSDAP he wrote in 1938: "I'm able to say that my whole scientific work is devoted to the ideas of the [[Nazism|National Socialists]]." His publications during that time led in later years to allegations that his scientific work had been contaminated by Nazi sympathies: his published writing during the Nazi period included support for Nazi ideas of "[[racial hygiene]]" couched in pseudoscientific metaphors.
Konrad Lorenz was born in ''Altenberg'', near [[Vienna]], [[Austria]], to ''Adolf Lorenz'' and ''Emma Lecher Lorenz'', both medical doctors. Always fascinated with animals, he kept a menagerie at home. His father insisted that he became a physician. At the request of his father, Konrad Lorenz began a premedical curriculum in [[1922]] at [[Columbia University]]. He returned to Vienna in [[1923]] to continue his studies at the [[University of Vienna]] until [[1928]]. Konrad Lorenz received his medical degree in [[1928]], from the [[University of Vienna]]. A year before that, he married ''Margarethe Gebhart'', also a doctor. Then studying at ''Ferdinand Hochstetter's Anatomical Institute'' at the University, he received his doctorate in [[zoology]], in [[1933]]. During his early period, he set up many of his hypotheses on animal behavior, such as [[imprinting]], ''innate releasing mechanism'', and ''fixed action patterns''. After serving as an assistant in the Anatomical Institute, Lorenz was a ''Privatdozent'' (unpaid instructor) at the [[University of Vienna]] from [[1937]] to [[1940]].
 
  
During 1930s, Lorenz established the major theoretical foundations of classical ethology. His basic insight, shared with predecessors and teachers, was that some instinctive behavior patterns were fixed in form and just as characteristic of species as organs. Lorenz planned to analyse instinctive behavior using comparative techniques. Lorenz joined the [[National Socialist German Workers Party|Nazi Party]] in [[1938]] and accepted a university chair under the [[Nazi Germany|Nazi regime]]. In his application for membership to the Nazi-party [[National Socialist German Workers Party|NSDAP]] he wrote in [[1938]]: "I'm able to say that my whole scientific work is devoted to the ideas of the [[Nazism|National Socialists]]." <!-- full text cited in de.Wikipedia —> His publications during that time led in later years to allegations that his scientific work had been contaminated by Nazi sympathies: his published writing during the Nazi period included support for Nazi ideas of "[[racial hygiene]]" couched in pseudoscientific metaphors.
+
In 1940, Lorenz became a professor of [[psychology]] at the [[Immanuel Kant University]] in Königsberg (later the [[Russia|Russian]] [[port]] of [[Kaliningrad]]). He was drafted into the [[Wehrmacht]] in 1941. He sought to be a motorcycle mechanic, but instead he was assigned as a medic. He was a [[prisoner of war]] in the Soviet Union from 1944 to 1948. The [[Max Planck Society]] established the Lorenz Institute for Behavioral Physiology in [[Buldern]], [[Germany]], in 1950. In 1958, Lorenz transferred to the [[Max Planck|Max Planck Institute for Behavioral Physiology]] in [[Seewiesen]]. He shared the 1973 [[Nobel Prize]] in Physiology or Medicine "for discoveries in individual and social behavior patterns" with two other important early ethologists, [[Nikolaas Tinbergen]] and [[Karl von Frisch]]. In 1969, he became the first recipient of the [[Prix mondial Cino Del Duca]]. Lorenz retired from the Max Planck Institute in 1973 but continued to research and publish from [[Altenberg, Austria|Altenberg]] (his family home, near Vienna) and [[Grünau im Almtal]] in Austria.  
 
 
In [[1940]], Lorenz became a professor of [[psychology]] at the [[Immanuel Kant University]] in Königsberg (later the [[Russia|Russian]] [[port]] of [[Kaliningrad]]). He was drafted into the [[Wehrmacht]] in [[1941]]. He sought to be a motorcycle mechanic, but instead he was assigned as a medic. He was a [[prisoner of war]] in the Soviet Union from [[1944]] to [[1948]]. The [[Max Planck Society]] established the Lorenz Institute for Behavioral Physiology in [[Buldern]], [[Germany]], in [[1950]]. In [[1958]], Lorenz transferred to the [[Max Planck|Max Planck Institute for Behavioral Physiology]] in [[Seewiesen]]. He shared the [[1973]] [[Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine]] "for discoveries in individual and social behavior patterns" with two other important early ethologists, [[Nikolaas Tinbergen]] and [[Karl von Frisch]]. In 1969, he became the first recipient of the [[Prix mondial Cino Del Duca]]. Lorenz retired from the Max Planck Institute in [[1973]] but continued to research and publish from [[Altenberg, Austria|Altenberg]] (his family home, near Vienna) and [[Grünau im Almtal]] in Austria.  
 
  
 
When accepting the Nobel Prize, he apologized for a 1940 publication that included Nazi views of science, saying that "many highly decent scientists hoped, like I did, for a short time for good from [[National Socialism]], and many quickly turned away from it with the same horror as I." It seems highly likely that Lorenz's ideas about an inherited basis for behavior patterns were congenial to the [[Nazi]] authorities, but there is no evidence to suggest that his experimental work was either inspired or distorted by Nazi ideas.
 
When accepting the Nobel Prize, he apologized for a 1940 publication that included Nazi views of science, saying that "many highly decent scientists hoped, like I did, for a short time for good from [[National Socialism]], and many quickly turned away from it with the same horror as I." It seems highly likely that Lorenz's ideas about an inherited basis for behavior patterns were congenial to the [[Nazi]] authorities, but there is no evidence to suggest that his experimental work was either inspired or distorted by Nazi ideas.
Line 21: Line 37:
 
Konrad Lorenz died on February 27, 1989, in Altenberg.
 
Konrad Lorenz died on February 27, 1989, in Altenberg.
  
= Work =
+
== Work ==
In several seminal papers, Lorenz set forth the theoretical concerns, findings, investigatory methods, and remaining problems of [[Ethology]]. Lorenz's methods were always conventional. He never did a formal experiment, and his descriptive observstions were often anecdotical. He infuriated his more conventional colleagues by saying, "If I have one good example, I  don't give a fig for statistics." By this he meant that if he had seen an animal do something striking, he did not need to see a lot of other animals do the same thing to confirm what he already knew. His doctrine of [[imprinting]] is still in focus of research interest until now. He loved animals and kept an enormous variety, including jackdows, geese, dogs, and fish.
+
In several seminal papers, Lorenz set forth the theoretical concerns, findings, investigatory methods, and remaining problems of [[Ethology]]. Lorenz's methods were always conventional. He never did a formal experiment, and his descriptive observstions were often anecdotical. He infuriated his more conventional colleagues by saying, "If I have one good example, I  don't give a fig for statistics." By this he meant that if he had seen an animal do something striking, he did not need to see a lot of other animals do the same thing to confirm what he already knew. His doctrine of [[imprinting (psychology)|imprinting]] is still in focus of research interest until now. He loved animals and kept an enormous variety, including [[jackdaw]]s, [[goose|geese]], [[dog]]s, and [[fish]].
 
+
[[Image:V31-d-Graugans.JPG|right|thumb|200px|The egg-rolling behavior of the greylag goose is a widely cited example of a ''fixed-action pattern'', one of the key concepts used by ethologists to explain animal behavior.]]
 
Konrad Lorenz has discovered a few concepts still being used in contemporary behavioral sciences:
 
Konrad Lorenz has discovered a few concepts still being used in contemporary behavioral sciences:
1) ''The fixed action pattern'' an inharited characteristic, relatively complex movement pattern shared by all members of a species;
+
1) ''The fixed action pattern'' an inharited characteristic, relatively complex movement pattern shared by all members of a species;
2) ''The releaser'' or ''sign stimulus'' a specific environmental stimulus, the perception of which is sufficient to cause the performance of a fixed action pattern;
+
2) ''The releaser'' or ''sign stimulus'' a specific environmental stimulus, the perception of which is sufficient to cause the performance of a fixed action pattern;
3) [[Imprinting]], a rapid, quite stable form of learning in which a social animal's early experiences affect its later social or sexual behavior, apparently without the need for [[reinforcement]].
+
3) [[imprinting (psychology)|imprinting]], a rapid, quite stable form of learning in which a social animal's early experiences affect its later social or sexual behavior, apparently without the need for [[reinforcement]].
  
The comparative study of human and animal mentation was dominant theme throughout Lorenz's work. He argued that perceptual and cognitive apparatus could be tteated like other organs that had evolved to the environment. Even the "disposition to learn" items crucial to survival could be soexplained. [[Evolutionary psychology]] and [[sociobiology]] are among the contemporary developments that build on these foundations. Lorenz always believed that humanity was in danger from the allegedly deleterious effects of civilization. Lorenz hypothesized varied causes for these effects, successsfully postulating racial degeneration, unhibited aggression, and environmental degredation during his career. In his book ''On Aggression'' (1966), Lorenz claimed that intraspecies aggression, normally an adaptive phenomenon in animals, has turned deadly in humans because our development of new weapons that can kill at a distance (which has outrun our innate inhibitions against killing). According to Lorenz, aggression involves stored instinctive energy and needs to be discharged. Then follows a refactory phase to build up the energy that has been flushed much like the flushing and refilling of a toilet. This approach of Lorenz incited an international debate on the biological bases of human behavior.
+
The comparative study of human and animal mentation was dominant theme throughout Lorenz's work. He argued that perceptual and cognitive apparatus could be treated like other organs that had evolved to the environment. Even the "disposition to learn" items crucial to survival could be so explained. [[Evolutionary psychology]] and [[sociobiology]] are among the contemporary developments that build on these foundations. Lorenz always believed that humanity was in danger from the allegedly deleterious effects of civilization. Lorenz hypothesized varied causes for these effects, successfully postulating racial degeneration, uninhibited aggression, and environmental degradation during his career. In his book ''On Aggression'' (1966), Lorenz claimed that intraspecies aggression, normally an adaptive phenomenon in animals, has turned deadly in humans because our development of new weapons that can kill at a distance (which has outrun our innate inhibitions against killing). According to Lorenz, aggression involves stored instinctive energy and needs to be discharged. Then follows a refactory phase to build up the energy that has been flushed much like the flushing and refilling of a toilet. This approach of Lorenz incited an international debate on the biological bases of human behavior.
  
 
Konrad Lorenz has also contributed to [[philosophy]]. In his book ''Behind the Mirror'', Lorenz considers the old philosophical question of whether our senses correctly inform us about the world as it is, or provide us only with an illusion. His answer comes from [[Evolutionary Biology]]. Only traits that help us survive and reproduce are transmitted. If our senses gave us wrong information about our environment, we would soon be extinct. Therefore we can be sure that our senses give us correct information, for otherwise we would not be here to be deceived.
 
Konrad Lorenz has also contributed to [[philosophy]]. In his book ''Behind the Mirror'', Lorenz considers the old philosophical question of whether our senses correctly inform us about the world as it is, or provide us only with an illusion. His answer comes from [[Evolutionary Biology]]. Only traits that help us survive and reproduce are transmitted. If our senses gave us wrong information about our environment, we would soon be extinct. Therefore we can be sure that our senses give us correct information, for otherwise we would not be here to be deceived.
Line 35: Line 51:
 
==Lorenz's vision on humankind==
 
==Lorenz's vision on humankind==
  
In his book ''Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins'', Lorenz addresses the following paradox: ''All the advantages that man has gained from his ever-deepening understanding of the natural world that surrounds him, his technological, chemical and medical progress, all of which should seem to alleviate human suffering... tends instead to favor humanity's destruction'' (p.26). Lorenz adopts an ecological model to attempt to grasp the mechanisms behind this contradiction. Thus ''all species... are adapted to their environment... including not only inorganic components... but all the other living beings that inhabit the locality'' (p. 31). Fundamental to Lorenz's theory of ecology is the function of feedback mechanisms, especially negative feedback mechanisms which, in hierarchical fashion, dampen impulses that occur beneath  a certain threshold. The thresholds themselves are the product of the interaction of contrasting mechanisms. Thus pain and pleasure act as checks on each other: ''To gain a desired prey, a dog or wolf will do things that, in other contexts, they would shy away from: run through thorn bushes, jump into cold water and expose themselves to risks which would normally frighten them. All these inhibitory mechanisms... act as a counterweight to the effects of learning mechanisms... The organism cannot allow itself to pay a price which is not worth the candle''(p. 53). In nature, these mechanisms tend toward a ''stable state'' among the living beings of an ecology. ''A closer examination shows that these beings... not only do not damage each other, but often constitute a community of interests. It is obvious that the predator is strongly interested in the survival of that species, animal or vegetable, which constitutes its prey. ... It is not uncommon that the prey species derives specific benefits from its interaction with the predator species...'' (p. 33). Lorenz asserts that humanity is the one species not bound by these mechanisms, being the one species which has defined its own environment: ''[The pace of human ecology] is determined by the progress of man's technology'' (p. 35). Not only, but  human ecology (economics) is governed by mechanisms of ''positive'' feedback, defined  as a mechanism which tends to encourage behavior rather than to attenuate it (p. 43). ''Positive feedback always involves the danger of an "avalanche" effect... One particular kind of positive feedback occurs when ''individuals of the same species'' enter into competition among themselves... For many animal species, environmental factors keep... intraspecies selection from [leading to] disaster... But there is no force which exercises this type of healthy regulatory effect on humanity's cultural development; unfortunately for itself, humanity has learned to overcome all those environmental forces which are external to itself'' (p. 44). Lorenz does not see human independence from natural ecological processes as necessarily bad. Indeed, he states that ''a completely new [ecology] which corresponds in every way to [humanity's] desires... could, theoretically, prove as durable as that which would have existed without his intervention'' (p. 36). However, the principle of competition, typical of Western societies, destroys any chance of this: ''The competition between human beings destroys with cold and diabolic brutality... Under the pressure of  this competitive fury we have not only forgotten what is useful to humanity as a whole, but even that which is good and advantageous to the individual. ... One asks, which is more damaging to modern  humanity: the thirst for money or consuming haste... in either case, fear plays a very important role: the fear of being overtaken by one's competitors, the fear of becoming poor, the fear of making wrong decisions or the fear of not being up to snuff...''(p. 45-47).
+
In his book ''Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins'', Lorenz addressed the following paradox: "All the advantages that man has gained from his ever-deepening understanding of the natural world that surrounds him, his technological, chemical and medical progress, all of which should seem to alleviate human suffering... tends instead to favor humanity's destruction" (p.26). Lorenz adopts an ecological model to attempt to grasp the mechanisms behind this contradiction. Thus "all species... are adapted to their environment... including not only inorganic components... but all the other living beings that inhabit the locality" (p. 31).  
 +
 
 +
Fundamental to Lorenz's theory of ecology is the function of feedback mechanisms, especially negative feedback mechanisms which, in hierarchical fashion, dampen impulses that occur beneath  a certain threshold. The thresholds themselves are the product of the interaction of contrasting mechanisms. Thus pain and pleasure act as checks on each other: "To gain a desired prey, a dog or wolf will do things that, in other contexts, they would shy away from: run through thorn bushes, jump into cold water and expose themselves to risks which would normally frighten them. All these inhibitory mechanisms... act as a counterweight to the effects of learning mechanisms... The organism cannot allow itself to pay a price which is not worth the candle"(p. 53).  
 +
 
 +
In nature, these mechanisms tend toward a ''stable state'' among the living beings of an ecology. "A closer examination shows that these beings... not only do not damage each other, but often constitute a community of interests. It is obvious that the predator is strongly interested in the survival of that species, animal or vegetable, which constitutes its prey. ... It is not uncommon that the prey species derives specific benefits from its interaction with the predator species" (p. 33).  
 +
 
 +
Lorenz asserts that humanity is the one species not bound by these mechanisms, being the one species which has defined its own environment: "[The pace of human ecology] is determined by the progress of man's technology" (p. 35). Not only, but  human ecology (economics) is governed by mechanisms of ''positive'' feedback, defined  as a mechanism which tends to encourage behavior rather than to attenuate it (p. 43).  
 +
<blockquote>Positive feedback always involves the danger of an "avalanche" effect... One particular kind of positive feedback occurs when individuals of the same species enter into competition among themselves... For many animal species, environmental factors keep... intraspecies selection from [leading to] disaster... But there is no force which exercises this type of healthy regulatory effect on humanity's cultural development; unfortunately for itself, humanity has learned to overcome all those environmental forces which are external to itself'' (p. 44).</blockquote>
 +
Lorenz does not see human independence from natural ecological processes as necessarily bad. Indeed, he states that "a completely new [ecology] which corresponds in every way to [humanity's] desires... could, theoretically, prove as durable as that which would have existed without his intervention" (p. 36). However, the principle of competition, typical of Western societies, destroys any chance of this:  
 +
<blockquote>The competition between human beings destroys with cold and diabolic brutality... Under the pressure of  this competitive fury we have not only forgotten what is useful to humanity as a whole, but even that which is good and advantageous to the individual. ... One asks, which is more damaging to modern  humanity: the thirst for money or consuming haste... in either case, fear plays a very important role: the fear of being overtaken by one's competitors, the fear of becoming poor, the fear of making wrong decisions or the fear of not being up to snuff(p. 45-47).</blockquote>
 +
 
 +
==Critique==
 +
Though Konrad Lorenz demonstrated that animal's [[instinctive behavior]] could be analysed through a repertoire of standard techniques, many of the details of his analyses of behavior occurred to be incomplete. Some would say that Lorenz' most significant contribution and legacy does not lie in any of his theories but in the good example he set with his methodology. He never deprived the animals of basic physical or emotional needs. He never killed them, mutilated them or tortured them. All these cruel methods were once considered indispensable for animal studies, but Lorenz proved it was possible to win a Nobel Prize without using them. Others would say that Lorenz's most enduring legacy was his almost prophetic vision of the relationship between market economics and the threat of ecological catastrophe.
  
= Legacy=
+
== Legacy==
 
Marc Bekoff (2006) has written about Konrad Lorenz, "I remember meeting Lorenz at an ethological conference in [[Parma]], [[Italy]], and his passion and enthusiasm were incredibly contagious. For hours, he told stories of the animals with whom he had shared his life and never once repeated himself. He clearly loved what he did and loved his animal friends."
 
Marc Bekoff (2006) has written about Konrad Lorenz, "I remember meeting Lorenz at an ethological conference in [[Parma]], [[Italy]], and his passion and enthusiasm were incredibly contagious. For hours, he told stories of the animals with whom he had shared his life and never once repeated himself. He clearly loved what he did and loved his animal friends."
  
Together with [[Nikolaas Tinbergen]], Lorenz developed the idea of an [[innate releasing mechanism]] to explain instinctive behaviors ([[fixed action pattern]]s). Influenced by the ideas of [[William McDougall]], Lorenz developed this into a "[[psychohydraulic]]" model of the [[motivation]] of behavior. These ideas were influential as ethology became more popular in the [[1960s]], but they are now regarded as [[outda]]  Lorenz's writings about [[evolution]] are also now regarded as outdated {{Fact|date=January 2007}}<!--- [[Wikipedia:Avoid weasel words#Examples]] --->, because he tended towards [[group selection]]ist ideas which have been heavily reinterpreted since the rise of [[sociobiology]] in the [[1970]]s. Lorenz's most enduring contributions thus seem to be his empirical work, especially on imprinting; his influence on a younger generation of ethologists; and his popular works, which were enormously important in bringing ethology to the attention of the general public.
+
Together with [[Nikolaas Tinbergen]], Lorenz developed the idea of an [[innate releasing mechanism]] to explain instinctive behaviors ([[fixed action pattern]]s). Influenced by the ideas of [[William McDougall]], Lorenz developed this into a "[[psychohydraulic]]" model of the [[motivation]] of behavior. These ideas were influential as ethology became more popular in the 1960s, but they are now regarded as [[outda]]  Lorenz's writings about [[evolution]] are also now regarded as outdated, because he tended towards [[group selection]]ist ideas which have been heavily reinterpreted since the rise of [[sociobiology]] in the 1970s. Lorenz's most enduring contributions thus seem to be his empirical work, especially on imprinting; his influence on a younger generation of ethologists; and his popular works, which were enormously important in bringing ethology to the attention of the general public.
  
There are three Konrad Lorenz Institutes in Austria; one of them is housed in his family mansion at Altenberg [http://www.kli.ac.at/].
+
There are three Konrad Lorenz Institutes in Austria; one of them is housed in his family mansion at Altenberg.
==Critique==
 
Though Konrad Lorenz demonstrated that animal's [[instinctive behavior]] could be analysed through a repertoire of standard techniques, many of the details of his analyses of behavior occurred to be incomplete. Some would say that Lorenz' most significant contribution and legacy does not lie in any of his theories but in the good example he set with his methodology. He never deprived the animals of basic physical or emotional needs. He never killed them, mutilated them or tortured them. All these cruel methods were once considered indispensable for animal studies, but Lorenz proved it was possible to win a Nobel Prize without using them. Others would say that Lorenz's most enduring legacy was his almost prophetic vision of the relationship between market economics and the threat of ecological catastrophy.
 
  
==Works==
+
==Publications==
Lorenz's best-known books are ''[[King Solomon's Ring (nonfiction)|King Solomon's Ring]]'' (1952) and ''[[On Aggression]]'' (1966), both written for a popular audience. His scientific work appeared mainly in journal articles, written in [[German language|German]]; they became widely known to English-speaking scientists through the descriptions of it in Tinbergen's [[1951 in science|1951 book]] ''The Study of Instinct'', though many of his papers were later published in English translation in the two volumes titled ''Studies in Animal and Human Behavior''.  
+
Lorenz's best-known books are ''King Solomon's Ring'' (1952) and ''On Aggression'' (1966), both written for a popular audience. His scientific work appeared mainly in journal articles, written in [[German language|German]]; they became widely known to English-speaking scientists through the descriptions of it in Tinbergen's 1951 book ''The Study of Instinct'', though many of his papers were later published in English translation in the two volumes titled ''Studies in Animal and Human Behavior''.  
  
*''[[King Solomon's Ring (nonfiction)|King Solomon's Ring]]'' ([[1952 in science|1952]])
+
*''King Solomon's Ring'' (1952)
*''[[Man Meets Dog]]'' ([[1954 in science|1954]])
+
*''Man Meets Dog'' (1954)
*''[[Evolution and Modification of Behavior]]'' ([[1965 in science|1965]])
+
*''Evolution and Modification of Behavior'' (1965)
*''[[On Aggression]]'' ([[1966 in science|1966]])
+
*''On Aggression'' (1966)
*''[[Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, Volume I]]'' ([[1970 in science|1970]])
+
*''Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, Volume I'' (1970)
*''[[Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, Volume II]]'' ([[1971 in science|1971]])
+
*''Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, Volume II'' (1971)
*''[[Behind the Mirror]]'' ([[1973 in science|1973]])
+
*''Behind the Mirror'' (1973)
*''[[Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins]]'' ([[1974 in science|1974]])
+
*''Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins'' (1974)
*''[[The Year of the Greyleg Goose]]'' ([[1979 in science|1979]])
+
*''The Year of the Greyleg Goose'' (1979)
*''[[The Foundations of Ethology]]'' ([[1982 in science|1982]])
+
*''The Foundations of Ethology'' (1982)
*''[[The Natural Science of the Human Species: An Introduction to Comparative Behavioral Research - The Russian Manuscript (1944-1948)]]''([[1995 in science|1995]])
+
*''The Natural Science of the Human Species: An Introduction to Comparative Behavioral Research - The Russian Manuscript (1944-1948)''(1995)
  
 
==References==
 
==References==
Line 70: Line 96:
  
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
*[http://www.nobel.se/medicine/laureates/1973/lorenz-autobio.html Short Autobiography on the Nobel Foundation website]
+
*[http://www.nobel.se/medicine/laureates/1973/lorenz-autobio.html Autobiography on the Nobel Foundation website] Retrieved July 18, 2007.
*[http://www.ess.uwe.ac.uk/genocide/reviewstr18.htm Review of Biologists Under Hitler]
 
 
*Konrad Lorenz Institutes:
 
*Konrad Lorenz Institutes:
**[http://www.kli.ac.at/ The Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research]
+
**[http://www.kli.ac.at/ The Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research] located at his family mansion in Altenburg. Retrieved July 18, 2007.
**[http://www.univie.ac.at/zoology/nbs/gruenau Konrad Lorenz Research Station], [[Grünau im Almtal]]
+
**[http://www.univie.ac.at/zoology/nbs/gruenau Konrad Lorenz Research Station] Retrieved July 18, 2007.
**[http://www.oeaw.ac.at/klivv Konrad Lorenz Institute for Ethology]
+
**[http://www.oeaw.ac.at/klivv Konrad Lorenz Institute for Ethology] Retrieved July 18, 2007.
  
 
{{Credit1|Konrad_Lorenz|101982843|}}
 
{{Credit1|Konrad_Lorenz|101982843|}}

Revision as of 19:54, 18 July 2007


Konrad Lorenz

Konrad lorenz-rusky lagr 1944-fotka.jpg
Born

November 7, 1903
Austria-Hungary flag 1869-1918.svg Vienna, Austria-Hungary

Died February 27, 1989

Flag of Austria Vienna, Austria

Residence Austria, Germany
Nationality Austrian
Field Zoologist, ethologist
Alma mater University of Vienna
Known for Imprinting
Notable prizes Nobel.svg Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine (1973)

Konrad Zacharias Lorenz (November 7, 1903 – February 27, 1989) was an Austrian zoologist, animal psychologist, and ornithologist. He is considered one of the founders of modern ethology. Lorenz studied instinctive behavior in animals, especially in greylag geese and jackdaws. Working with geese, he re-discovered the principle of imprinting (originally described by Douglas Spalding in the 19th century) in the behavior of nidifugous birds.

Life

Konrad Zacharias Lorenz was born in Altenberg, near Vienna, Austria, to Adolf Lorenz and Emma Lecher Lorenz, both medical doctors. Always fascinated with animals, he kept a menagerie at home. His father insisted that he became a physician. At the request of his father, Konrad Lorenz began a premedical curriculum in 1922 at Columbia University. He returned to Vienna in 1923 to continue his studies at the University of Vienna until 1928. Konrad Lorenz received his medical degree in 1928, from the University of Vienna. A year before that, he married Margarethe Gebhart, also a doctor. Then studying at Ferdinand Hochstetter's Anatomical Institute at the University, he received his doctorate in zoology, in 1933. During his early period, he set up many of his hypotheses on animal behavior, such as imprinting, innate releasing mechanism, and fixed action patterns. After serving as an assistant in the Anatomical Institute, Lorenz was a Privatdozent (unpaid instructor) at the University of Vienna from 1937 to 1940.

During 1930s, Lorenz established the major theoretical foundations of classical ethology. His basic insight, shared with predecessors and teachers, was that some instinctive behavior patterns were fixed in form and just as characteristic of species as organs. Lorenz planned to analyse instinctive behavior using comparative techniques. Lorenz joined the Nazi Party in 1938 and accepted a university chair under the Nazi regime. In his application for membership to the Nazi-party NSDAP he wrote in 1938: "I'm able to say that my whole scientific work is devoted to the ideas of the National Socialists." His publications during that time led in later years to allegations that his scientific work had been contaminated by Nazi sympathies: his published writing during the Nazi period included support for Nazi ideas of "racial hygiene" couched in pseudoscientific metaphors.

In 1940, Lorenz became a professor of psychology at the Immanuel Kant University in Königsberg (later the Russian port of Kaliningrad). He was drafted into the Wehrmacht in 1941. He sought to be a motorcycle mechanic, but instead he was assigned as a medic. He was a prisoner of war in the Soviet Union from 1944 to 1948. The Max Planck Society established the Lorenz Institute for Behavioral Physiology in Buldern, Germany, in 1950. In 1958, Lorenz transferred to the Max Planck Institute for Behavioral Physiology in Seewiesen. He shared the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine "for discoveries in individual and social behavior patterns" with two other important early ethologists, Nikolaas Tinbergen and Karl von Frisch. In 1969, he became the first recipient of the Prix mondial Cino Del Duca. Lorenz retired from the Max Planck Institute in 1973 but continued to research and publish from Altenberg (his family home, near Vienna) and Grünau im Almtal in Austria.

When accepting the Nobel Prize, he apologized for a 1940 publication that included Nazi views of science, saying that "many highly decent scientists hoped, like I did, for a short time for good from National Socialism, and many quickly turned away from it with the same horror as I." It seems highly likely that Lorenz's ideas about an inherited basis for behavior patterns were congenial to the Nazi authorities, but there is no evidence to suggest that his experimental work was either inspired or distorted by Nazi ideas.

During the final years of his life Lorenz supported the fledgling Austrian Green Party and in 1984 became the figurehead of the Konrad Lorenz Volksbegehren, a grass-roots movement that was formed to prevent the building of a power plant at the Danube near Hainburg an der Donau and thus the destruction of the yet untouched woodland surrounding the planned site.

Konrad Lorenz died on February 27, 1989, in Altenberg.

Work

In several seminal papers, Lorenz set forth the theoretical concerns, findings, investigatory methods, and remaining problems of Ethology. Lorenz's methods were always conventional. He never did a formal experiment, and his descriptive observstions were often anecdotical. He infuriated his more conventional colleagues by saying, "If I have one good example, I don't give a fig for statistics." By this he meant that if he had seen an animal do something striking, he did not need to see a lot of other animals do the same thing to confirm what he already knew. His doctrine of imprinting is still in focus of research interest until now. He loved animals and kept an enormous variety, including jackdaws, geese, dogs, and fish.

The egg-rolling behavior of the greylag goose is a widely cited example of a fixed-action pattern, one of the key concepts used by ethologists to explain animal behavior.

Konrad Lorenz has discovered a few concepts still being used in contemporary behavioral sciences: 1) The fixed action pattern — an inharited characteristic, relatively complex movement pattern shared by all members of a species; 2) The releaser or sign stimulus — a specific environmental stimulus, the perception of which is sufficient to cause the performance of a fixed action pattern; 3) imprinting, a rapid, quite stable form of learning in which a social animal's early experiences affect its later social or sexual behavior, apparently without the need for reinforcement.

The comparative study of human and animal mentation was dominant theme throughout Lorenz's work. He argued that perceptual and cognitive apparatus could be treated like other organs that had evolved to the environment. Even the "disposition to learn" items crucial to survival could be so explained. Evolutionary psychology and sociobiology are among the contemporary developments that build on these foundations. Lorenz always believed that humanity was in danger from the allegedly deleterious effects of civilization. Lorenz hypothesized varied causes for these effects, successfully postulating racial degeneration, uninhibited aggression, and environmental degradation during his career. In his book On Aggression (1966), Lorenz claimed that intraspecies aggression, normally an adaptive phenomenon in animals, has turned deadly in humans because our development of new weapons that can kill at a distance (which has outrun our innate inhibitions against killing). According to Lorenz, aggression involves stored instinctive energy and needs to be discharged. Then follows a refactory phase to build up the energy that has been flushed much like the flushing and refilling of a toilet. This approach of Lorenz incited an international debate on the biological bases of human behavior.

Konrad Lorenz has also contributed to philosophy. In his book Behind the Mirror, Lorenz considers the old philosophical question of whether our senses correctly inform us about the world as it is, or provide us only with an illusion. His answer comes from Evolutionary Biology. Only traits that help us survive and reproduce are transmitted. If our senses gave us wrong information about our environment, we would soon be extinct. Therefore we can be sure that our senses give us correct information, for otherwise we would not be here to be deceived.

Lorenz's vision on humankind

In his book Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins, Lorenz addressed the following paradox: "All the advantages that man has gained from his ever-deepening understanding of the natural world that surrounds him, his technological, chemical and medical progress, all of which should seem to alleviate human suffering... tends instead to favor humanity's destruction" (p.26). Lorenz adopts an ecological model to attempt to grasp the mechanisms behind this contradiction. Thus "all species... are adapted to their environment... including not only inorganic components... but all the other living beings that inhabit the locality" (p. 31).

Fundamental to Lorenz's theory of ecology is the function of feedback mechanisms, especially negative feedback mechanisms which, in hierarchical fashion, dampen impulses that occur beneath a certain threshold. The thresholds themselves are the product of the interaction of contrasting mechanisms. Thus pain and pleasure act as checks on each other: "To gain a desired prey, a dog or wolf will do things that, in other contexts, they would shy away from: run through thorn bushes, jump into cold water and expose themselves to risks which would normally frighten them. All these inhibitory mechanisms... act as a counterweight to the effects of learning mechanisms... The organism cannot allow itself to pay a price which is not worth the candle"(p. 53).

In nature, these mechanisms tend toward a stable state among the living beings of an ecology. "A closer examination shows that these beings... not only do not damage each other, but often constitute a community of interests. It is obvious that the predator is strongly interested in the survival of that species, animal or vegetable, which constitutes its prey. ... It is not uncommon that the prey species derives specific benefits from its interaction with the predator species" (p. 33).

Lorenz asserts that humanity is the one species not bound by these mechanisms, being the one species which has defined its own environment: "[The pace of human ecology] is determined by the progress of man's technology" (p. 35). Not only, but human ecology (economics) is governed by mechanisms of positive feedback, defined as a mechanism which tends to encourage behavior rather than to attenuate it (p. 43).

Positive feedback always involves the danger of an "avalanche" effect... One particular kind of positive feedback occurs when individuals of the same species enter into competition among themselves... For many animal species, environmental factors keep... intraspecies selection from [leading to] disaster... But there is no force which exercises this type of healthy regulatory effect on humanity's cultural development; unfortunately for itself, humanity has learned to overcome all those environmental forces which are external to itself (p. 44).

Lorenz does not see human independence from natural ecological processes as necessarily bad. Indeed, he states that "a completely new [ecology] which corresponds in every way to [humanity's] desires... could, theoretically, prove as durable as that which would have existed without his intervention" (p. 36). However, the principle of competition, typical of Western societies, destroys any chance of this:

The competition between human beings destroys with cold and diabolic brutality... Under the pressure of this competitive fury we have not only forgotten what is useful to humanity as a whole, but even that which is good and advantageous to the individual. ... One asks, which is more damaging to modern humanity: the thirst for money or consuming haste... in either case, fear plays a very important role: the fear of being overtaken by one's competitors, the fear of becoming poor, the fear of making wrong decisions or the fear of not being up to snuff(p. 45-47).

Critique

Though Konrad Lorenz demonstrated that animal's instinctive behavior could be analysed through a repertoire of standard techniques, many of the details of his analyses of behavior occurred to be incomplete. Some would say that Lorenz' most significant contribution and legacy does not lie in any of his theories but in the good example he set with his methodology. He never deprived the animals of basic physical or emotional needs. He never killed them, mutilated them or tortured them. All these cruel methods were once considered indispensable for animal studies, but Lorenz proved it was possible to win a Nobel Prize without using them. Others would say that Lorenz's most enduring legacy was his almost prophetic vision of the relationship between market economics and the threat of ecological catastrophe.

Legacy

Marc Bekoff (2006) has written about Konrad Lorenz, "I remember meeting Lorenz at an ethological conference in Parma, Italy, and his passion and enthusiasm were incredibly contagious. For hours, he told stories of the animals with whom he had shared his life and never once repeated himself. He clearly loved what he did and loved his animal friends."

Together with Nikolaas Tinbergen, Lorenz developed the idea of an innate releasing mechanism to explain instinctive behaviors (fixed action patterns). Influenced by the ideas of William McDougall, Lorenz developed this into a "psychohydraulic" model of the motivation of behavior. These ideas were influential as ethology became more popular in the 1960s, but they are now regarded as outda Lorenz's writings about evolution are also now regarded as outdated, because he tended towards group selectionist ideas which have been heavily reinterpreted since the rise of sociobiology in the 1970s. Lorenz's most enduring contributions thus seem to be his empirical work, especially on imprinting; his influence on a younger generation of ethologists; and his popular works, which were enormously important in bringing ethology to the attention of the general public.

There are three Konrad Lorenz Institutes in Austria; one of them is housed in his family mansion at Altenberg.

Publications

Lorenz's best-known books are King Solomon's Ring (1952) and On Aggression (1966), both written for a popular audience. His scientific work appeared mainly in journal articles, written in German; they became widely known to English-speaking scientists through the descriptions of it in Tinbergen's 1951 book The Study of Instinct, though many of his papers were later published in English translation in the two volumes titled Studies in Animal and Human Behavior.

  • King Solomon's Ring (1952)
  • Man Meets Dog (1954)
  • Evolution and Modification of Behavior (1965)
  • On Aggression (1966)
  • Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, Volume I (1970)
  • Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, Volume II (1971)
  • Behind the Mirror (1973)
  • Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins (1974)
  • The Year of the Greyleg Goose (1979)
  • The Foundations of Ethology (1982)
  • The Natural Science of the Human Species: An Introduction to Comparative Behavioral Research - The Russian Manuscript (1944-1948)(1995)

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Bekoff, M., Animal Passions and Beastly Virtues" (2006), ISBN 1-59213-347-9
  • Boring, E.G. 1950. A history of experimental psychology, 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. ISBN 0133900398
  • Brennan, J.F. 1986. History and systems of psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc. ISBN 0133922189
  • Leahey, Th. H. 1991. A History of Modern Psychology. Englewood Cliff, NJ: Prentice Hall. 3rd edition. 2000. ISBN 0130175730
  • Lorenz, K. Z. 1975 (Originally published in 1941). Kant's doctrine of the a priori in the light of contemporary biology. In Richard I. Evans, Konrad Lorenz: The man and his ideas. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
  • Lorenz, K. Z. 1957 (Originally published in 1939). Comparative study of behavior. In C.H. Schiller (Ed. and Trans.). Instinctive behavior: The development of a modern concept. London: Methuen.

External links

Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.