Difference between revisions of "Sight" - New World Encyclopedia

From New World Encyclopedia
({{Contracted}})
Line 1: Line 1:
 
{{Claimed}}{{Contracted}}
 
{{Claimed}}{{Contracted}}
 +
 +
[[Image:eye.gif|thumb|right|120px|Eye]]
 +
 +
'''[[Sight]]''' or '''vision''' describes the capability to detect electromagnetic energy within the visible range ([[light]]) by the [[eye]], and the ability of the [[brain]] to interpret the image as "sight." There is disagreement as to whether this constitutes one, two or even three distinct senses. Neuroanatomists generally regard it as two senses, given that different receptors are responsible for the perception of color (the frequency of photons of light) and brightness (amplitude/intensity - number of photons of light). Some argue that the perception of depth also constitutes a sense, but it is generally regarded that this is really a cognitive (that is, post-sensory) function of brain to interpret sensory input to derive new information.
 +
 +
example: Olfaction, the sense of smell, is the detection of chemicals dissolved in air. It is one of the five senses originally described by Aristotle.
 +
 +
example: Hearing (or audition) is the sense of detecting sound, that is, receiving information about the environment from vibratory movement communicated through a medium such as air, water, or ground. It is one of the traditional five senses, along with sight, touch, smell, and taste.
 +
 +
In [[psychology]], '''visual perception''' is the ability to interpret [[visible light]] information reaching the [[eye]]s. The resulting [[perception]] is also known as '''eyesight''', '''sight''' or '''vision'''. The various components involved in vision are known as the [[visual system]].
 +
 +
==Overview==
 +
Broadly defined, a '''sense''' is a mechanism or faculty by which a living organism receives information about its external or internal environment. In other words, it is an inherent capability or power to receive and process stimuli from outside and inside the body, such as the sense of hearing or sense of sight.
 +
 +
As defined, this term includes both physiological methods of perception, involving reception of stimuli by sensory cells, and incorporeal methods of perception, as in spiritual senses (see [[sense#Spiritual senses|spiritual senses]] below). It also allows sense by microorganisms, such as signal transduction pathways that allow detection of nutrients, including sugars, amino acides, and nitorgen sources. ''Geobacter metallireducens'', a common soil bacterium, is reported to have a built-in sensor that allows it to locate iron and manganese oxides that serves as its food source.
 +
 +
The term sense is often more narrowly defined, as related to higher animals. In this case, a sense is considered a system involving sensory cells that respond to a specific kind of physical energy (both internal and external stimuli), which are converted into nerve impulses that travel to the brain (either a specialized area of the brain or specialized areas), where the signals are received and analyzed.
 +
 +
School children are routinely taught that there are five senses (sight, hearing, touch, smell, taste; a classification first devised by [[Aristotle]]), but it is generally agreed that there are at least nine different senses in humans, and a minimum of two more observed in other organisms.
 +
 +
The senses and their operation, classification, and theory are overlapping topics studied by a variety of fields, most notably [[neuroscience]], [[cognitive psychology]] (or [[cognitive science]]), and [[philosophy of perception]].
 +
 +
Depending on the chosen method of classification, somewhere between 9 and 21 human senses have been identified. In addition, there are some other candidate physiological experiences that may or may not fall within the above classification (for example the sensory awareness of hunger and thirst). In addition, many individuals report various spiritual senses. Finally, some individuals report [[synesthesia]], the "crossing-over" of one sense to another, or even of senses associated with certain pure concepts. A synesthete may, for example, hear colors, see sounds, and taste shapes or tactile sensations.
 +
 +
===Spiritual senses===
 +
Throughout history, there have been many published reports of people having "spiritual senses." In this case, a '''spiritual sense''' refers to a mechanism or faculty to receive and process internal or external stimuli of an incorporeal nature. For example, there are references in sacred [[scripture]], as well as popular books and media, to individuals who see, hear or even touch relatives or others who have passed away.
 +
 +
==Visual system==
 +
{{main|Visual system}}
 +
 +
The [[visual system]] allows us to assimilate information from the environment to help guide our actions. The act of seeing starts when the [[lens (anatomy)|lens]] of the [[eye]] focus an image of the outside world onto a light-sensitive membrane in the back of the eye, called the [[retina]]. The retina is actually part of the [[brain]] that is isolated to serve as a [[transducer]] for the conversion of patterns of light into neuronal signals. The [[lens (anatomy)|lens]] of the [[eye]] focuses light on the [[photoreceptor cells|photoreceptive cells]] of the retina, which detect the [[photon]]s of light and respond by producing [[Action potential|neural impulses]]. These signals are processed in a [[Hierarchy|hierarchical]] fashion by different parts of the brain, from the retina to the [[lateral geniculate nucleus]], to the primary and secondary [[visual cortex]] of the [[brain]].
 +
 +
== Study of visual perception ==
 +
The major problem in visual perception is that what people see is not simply a translation of retinal stimuli (i.e., the image on the retina). Thus people interested in perception have long struggled to explain what visual processing does to create what we actually see.
 +
 +
=== Early studies on visual perception ===
 +
[[Image:Ventral-dorsal streams.svg|thumb|right|300px|The visual [[dorsal stream]] (green) and [[ventral stream]] (purple) are shown.  Much of the human [[cerebral cortex]] is involved in vision.]]
 +
 +
There were two major Grecian schools, providing a primitive explanation of how vision is carried out in the body.
 +
 +
The first was the "[[Emission theory (vision)|emission theory]]" which maintained that vision occurs when rays emanate from the eyes and are intercepted by visual objects. If we saw an object directly it was by 'means of rays' coming out of the eyes and again falling on the object. A refracted image was, however, seen by 'means of rays' as well, which came out of the eyes, traversed through the air, and after refraction, fell on the visible object which was sighted as the result of the movement of the rays from the eye. Although this theory was championed by scholars like [[Euclid]] and [[Ptolemy]] and their followers, it was believed by [[Descartes]].{{Fact|date=September 2007}}
 +
 +
The second school advocated the so called the 'intromission' approach which sees vision as coming from something entering the eyes representative of the object. With its main propagators Aristotle, Galen and their followers, this theory seems to have touched a little sense on what really vision is, but remained only a speculation lacking any experimental foundation.
 +
 +
The breakthrough came with [[Ibn al-Haytham]] (Alhacen), the "father of [[optics]]", who pioneered the [[science|scientific]] study of the psychology of visual perception in his influential ''[[Book of Optics]]'' in the [[1000s]], being the first [[scientist]] to argue that vision occurs in the brain, rather than the eyes. He pointed out that personal experience has an effect on what people see and how they see, and that vision and perception are subjective. He explained possible errors in vision in detail, and as an example, describes how a small child with less experience may have more difficulty interpreting what he/she sees. He also gives an example of an adult that can make mistakes in vision because of how one's experience suggests that he/she is seeing one thing, when he/she is really seeing something else.<ref name=Steffens>Bradley Steffens (2006). ''Ibn al-Haytham: First Scientist'', Chapter 5. Morgan Reynolds Publishing. ISBN 1599350246.</ref>
 +
 +
Ibn al-Haytham's investigations and [[experiment]]s on visual perception also included [[sensation]], variations in [[sensitivity]], sensation of [[touch]], [[perception]] of colours, perception of [[darkness]], the psychological explanation of the [[moon illusion]], and [[binocular vision]].<ref name=Howard>{{cite journal | author=Howard, I | title=Alhazen's neglected discoveries of visual phenomena | journal=Perception | volume=25 | pages=1203-1217 | year=1996}}</ref><ref name=Khaleefa>{{cite journal | author=Omar Khaleefa | year=1999 | title=Who Is the Founder of Psychophysics and Experimental Psychology? | journal=American Journal of Islamic Social Sciences | volume=16 | issue=2}}</ref>
 +
 +
===Unconscious inference===
 +
[[Image:Mirvseyes.jpg|thumb|right|Human eyes]]
 +
 +
[[Hermann von Helmholtz]] is often credited with the first study of visual perception in [[Modern Times|modern times]]. Helmholtz held vision to be a form of unconscious [[inference]]: vision is a matter of deriving a probable interpretation for incomplete data.
 +
 +
Inference requires prior assumptions about the world: two well-known assumptions that we make in processing visual information are that light comes from above, and that objects are viewed from above and not below. The study of [[visual illusions]] (cases when the inference process goes wrong) has yielded much insight into what sort of assumptions the visual system makes.
 +
 +
The unconscious inference hypothesis has recently been revived in so-called [[Bayesian]] studies of visual perception. Proponents of this approach consider that the visual system performs some form of [[Bayesian inference]] to derive a perception from sensory data. Models based on this idea have been used to describe various visual subsystems, such as the perception of [[motion (physics)|motion]] or the perception of [[depth perception|depth]].<ref>Mamassian, Landy & Maloney (2002)</ref><ref>[http://www.purveslab.net/research/ A Primer on Probabilistic Approaches to Visual Perception]</ref>
 +
 +
===Gestalt theory===
 +
{{main|Gestalt psychology}}
 +
[[Gestalt psychology|Gestalt psychologists]] working primarily in the 1930s and 1940s raised many of the research questions that are studied by vision scientists today.
 +
 +
The Gestalt '''Laws of Organization''' have guided the study of how people perceive visual components as organized patterns or wholes, instead of many different parts. Gestalt is a German word that translates to "configuration or pattern". According to this theory, there are six main factors that determine how we group things according to visual perception: Proximity, Similarity, Closure, Symmetry, Common fate and Continuity.
 +
 +
The major problem with the Gestalt laws (and the Gestalt school generally) is that they are ''descriptive'' not ''explanatory''. For example, one cannot explain how humans see continuous contours by simply stating that the brain "prefers good continuity". [[Computational neuroscience|Computational models]] of vision have had more success in explaining visual phenomena<ref name="Dakin">[http://www.ucl.ac.uk/~smgxscd/ContourResearch.html Computational models of contour integration, by Steve Dakin]</ref> and have largely superseded Gestalt theory.
 +
 +
*[[Color vision]]
 +
*[[Motion perception]]
 +
*[[Depth perception]]
 +
*[[Visual illusion]]
 +
 +
===Disorders/Dysfunctions===
 +
*[[Achromatopsia]]
 +
*[[Color blindness]]
 +
*[[Scotopic Sensitivity Syndrome]]
 +
 +
===Related Disciplines===
 +
*[[Psychophysics]]
 +
*[[Neuroscience]]
 +
*[[Cognitive Science]]
 +
*[[Optometry]]
 +
*[[Ophthalmology]]
 +
 +
==See also==
 +
{{CommonsCat|Sight}}
 +
 +
== References ==
 +
{{reflist}}
 +
 +
==External links==
 +
* [http://enane.de/cont.htm Empiristic theory of visual gestalt perception]
 +
* [http://www.aber.ac.uk/media/Modules/MC10220/visper03.html Visual Perception 3 - Cultural and Environmental Factors]
 +
* [http://www.sapdesignguild.org/resources/optical_illusions/gestalt_laws.html Gestalt Laws]
 +
*[http://develintel.blogspot.com/2006/01/kosslyns-cognitive-architecture.html Summary of Kosslyn et al.'s theory of high-level vision]
 +
* [http://webvision.med.utah.edu/ The Organization of the Retina and Visual System]
 +
*[http://dr.trippy.googlepages.com Dr Trippy's Sensorium] A  website dedicated to the study of the human sensorium and organisational behaviour
 +
 +
{{Sensory system}}
 +
 +
[[Category:Life sciences]]
 +
[[Category:Psychology]]
 +
{{credit|Visual_perception|172566815}}

Revision as of 14:55, 7 December 2007


Eye

Sight or vision describes the capability to detect electromagnetic energy within the visible range (light) by the eye, and the ability of the brain to interpret the image as "sight." There is disagreement as to whether this constitutes one, two or even three distinct senses. Neuroanatomists generally regard it as two senses, given that different receptors are responsible for the perception of color (the frequency of photons of light) and brightness (amplitude/intensity - number of photons of light). Some argue that the perception of depth also constitutes a sense, but it is generally regarded that this is really a cognitive (that is, post-sensory) function of brain to interpret sensory input to derive new information.

example: Olfaction, the sense of smell, is the detection of chemicals dissolved in air. It is one of the five senses originally described by Aristotle.

example: Hearing (or audition) is the sense of detecting sound, that is, receiving information about the environment from vibratory movement communicated through a medium such as air, water, or ground. It is one of the traditional five senses, along with sight, touch, smell, and taste.

In psychology, visual perception is the ability to interpret visible light information reaching the eyes. The resulting perception is also known as eyesight, sight or vision. The various components involved in vision are known as the visual system.

Overview

Broadly defined, a sense is a mechanism or faculty by which a living organism receives information about its external or internal environment. In other words, it is an inherent capability or power to receive and process stimuli from outside and inside the body, such as the sense of hearing or sense of sight.

As defined, this term includes both physiological methods of perception, involving reception of stimuli by sensory cells, and incorporeal methods of perception, as in spiritual senses (see spiritual senses below). It also allows sense by microorganisms, such as signal transduction pathways that allow detection of nutrients, including sugars, amino acides, and nitorgen sources. Geobacter metallireducens, a common soil bacterium, is reported to have a built-in sensor that allows it to locate iron and manganese oxides that serves as its food source.

The term sense is often more narrowly defined, as related to higher animals. In this case, a sense is considered a system involving sensory cells that respond to a specific kind of physical energy (both internal and external stimuli), which are converted into nerve impulses that travel to the brain (either a specialized area of the brain or specialized areas), where the signals are received and analyzed.

School children are routinely taught that there are five senses (sight, hearing, touch, smell, taste; a classification first devised by Aristotle), but it is generally agreed that there are at least nine different senses in humans, and a minimum of two more observed in other organisms.

The senses and their operation, classification, and theory are overlapping topics studied by a variety of fields, most notably neuroscience, cognitive psychology (or cognitive science), and philosophy of perception.

Depending on the chosen method of classification, somewhere between 9 and 21 human senses have been identified. In addition, there are some other candidate physiological experiences that may or may not fall within the above classification (for example the sensory awareness of hunger and thirst). In addition, many individuals report various spiritual senses. Finally, some individuals report synesthesia, the "crossing-over" of one sense to another, or even of senses associated with certain pure concepts. A synesthete may, for example, hear colors, see sounds, and taste shapes or tactile sensations.

Spiritual senses

Throughout history, there have been many published reports of people having "spiritual senses." In this case, a spiritual sense refers to a mechanism or faculty to receive and process internal or external stimuli of an incorporeal nature. For example, there are references in sacred scripture, as well as popular books and media, to individuals who see, hear or even touch relatives or others who have passed away.

Visual system

The visual system allows us to assimilate information from the environment to help guide our actions. The act of seeing starts when the lens of the eye focus an image of the outside world onto a light-sensitive membrane in the back of the eye, called the retina. The retina is actually part of the brain that is isolated to serve as a transducer for the conversion of patterns of light into neuronal signals. The lens of the eye focuses light on the photoreceptive cells of the retina, which detect the photons of light and respond by producing neural impulses. These signals are processed in a hierarchical fashion by different parts of the brain, from the retina to the lateral geniculate nucleus, to the primary and secondary visual cortex of the brain.

Study of visual perception

The major problem in visual perception is that what people see is not simply a translation of retinal stimuli (i.e., the image on the retina). Thus people interested in perception have long struggled to explain what visual processing does to create what we actually see.

Early studies on visual perception

The visual dorsal stream (green) and ventral stream (purple) are shown. Much of the human cerebral cortex is involved in vision.

There were two major Grecian schools, providing a primitive explanation of how vision is carried out in the body.

The first was the "emission theory" which maintained that vision occurs when rays emanate from the eyes and are intercepted by visual objects. If we saw an object directly it was by 'means of rays' coming out of the eyes and again falling on the object. A refracted image was, however, seen by 'means of rays' as well, which came out of the eyes, traversed through the air, and after refraction, fell on the visible object which was sighted as the result of the movement of the rays from the eye. Although this theory was championed by scholars like Euclid and Ptolemy and their followers, it was believed by Descartes.[citation needed]

The second school advocated the so called the 'intromission' approach which sees vision as coming from something entering the eyes representative of the object. With its main propagators Aristotle, Galen and their followers, this theory seems to have touched a little sense on what really vision is, but remained only a speculation lacking any experimental foundation.

The breakthrough came with Ibn al-Haytham (Alhacen), the "father of optics", who pioneered the scientific study of the psychology of visual perception in his influential Book of Optics in the 1000s, being the first scientist to argue that vision occurs in the brain, rather than the eyes. He pointed out that personal experience has an effect on what people see and how they see, and that vision and perception are subjective. He explained possible errors in vision in detail, and as an example, describes how a small child with less experience may have more difficulty interpreting what he/she sees. He also gives an example of an adult that can make mistakes in vision because of how one's experience suggests that he/she is seeing one thing, when he/she is really seeing something else.[1]

Ibn al-Haytham's investigations and experiments on visual perception also included sensation, variations in sensitivity, sensation of touch, perception of colours, perception of darkness, the psychological explanation of the moon illusion, and binocular vision.[2][3]

Unconscious inference

Human eyes

Hermann von Helmholtz is often credited with the first study of visual perception in modern times. Helmholtz held vision to be a form of unconscious inference: vision is a matter of deriving a probable interpretation for incomplete data.

Inference requires prior assumptions about the world: two well-known assumptions that we make in processing visual information are that light comes from above, and that objects are viewed from above and not below. The study of visual illusions (cases when the inference process goes wrong) has yielded much insight into what sort of assumptions the visual system makes.

The unconscious inference hypothesis has recently been revived in so-called Bayesian studies of visual perception. Proponents of this approach consider that the visual system performs some form of Bayesian inference to derive a perception from sensory data. Models based on this idea have been used to describe various visual subsystems, such as the perception of motion or the perception of depth.[4][5]

Gestalt theory

Gestalt psychologists working primarily in the 1930s and 1940s raised many of the research questions that are studied by vision scientists today.

The Gestalt Laws of Organization have guided the study of how people perceive visual components as organized patterns or wholes, instead of many different parts. Gestalt is a German word that translates to "configuration or pattern". According to this theory, there are six main factors that determine how we group things according to visual perception: Proximity, Similarity, Closure, Symmetry, Common fate and Continuity.

The major problem with the Gestalt laws (and the Gestalt school generally) is that they are descriptive not explanatory. For example, one cannot explain how humans see continuous contours by simply stating that the brain "prefers good continuity". Computational models of vision have had more success in explaining visual phenomena[6] and have largely superseded Gestalt theory.

Disorders/Dysfunctions

  • Achromatopsia
  • Color blindness
  • Scotopic Sensitivity Syndrome

Related Disciplines

  • Psychophysics
  • Neuroscience
  • Cognitive Science
  • Optometry
  • Ophthalmology

See also

Template:CommonsCat

References
ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  1. Bradley Steffens (2006). Ibn al-Haytham: First Scientist, Chapter 5. Morgan Reynolds Publishing. ISBN 1599350246.
  2. Howard, I (1996). Alhazen's neglected discoveries of visual phenomena. Perception 25: 1203-1217.
  3. Omar Khaleefa (1999). Who Is the Founder of Psychophysics and Experimental Psychology?. American Journal of Islamic Social Sciences 16 (2).
  4. Mamassian, Landy & Maloney (2002)
  5. A Primer on Probabilistic Approaches to Visual Perception
  6. Computational models of contour integration, by Steve Dakin

External links


Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.